901 resultados para tennis players
Resumo:
Carvalho, FLP, Carvalho, MCGA, Simao, R, Gomes, TM, Costa, PB, Neto, LB, Carvalho, RLP, and Dantas, EHM. Acute effects of a warm-up including active, passive, and dynamic stretching on vertical jump performance. J Strength Cond Res 26(9): 2447-2452, 2012-The purpose of this study was to examine the acute effects of 3 different stretching methods combined with a warm-up protocol on vertical jump performance. Sixteen young tennis players (14.5 +/- 2.8 years; 175 +/- 5.6 cm; 64.0 +/- 11.1 kg) were randomly assigned to 4 different experimental conditions on 4 successive days. Each session consisted of a general and specific warm-up, with 5 minutes of running followed by 10 jumps, accompanied by one of the subsequent conditions: (a) Control Condition (CC)-5 minutes of passive rest; (b) Passive Stretching Condition (PSC)-5 minutes of passive static stretching; (c) Active Stretching Condition (ASC)-5 minutes of active static stretching; and (d) Dynamic Stretching Condition (DC)-5 minutes of dynamic stretching. After each intervention, the subjects performed 3 squat jumps (SJs) and 3 countermovement jumps (CMJs), which were measured electronically. For the SJ, 1-way repeated measures analysis of variance (CC x PSC x ASC x DC) revealed significant decreases for ASC (28.7 +/- 4.7 cm; p = 0.01) and PSC (28.7 +/- 4.3 cm; p = 0.02) conditions when compared with CC (29.9 +/- 5.0 cm). For CMJs, there were no significant decreases (p > 0.05) when all stretching conditions were compared with the CC. Significant increases in SJ performance were observed when comparing the DC (29.6 +/- 4.9 cm; p = 0.02) with PSC (28.7 +/- 4.3 cm). Significant increases in CMJ performance were observed when comparing the conditions ASC (34.0 +/- 6.0 cm; p = 0.04) and DC (33.7 +/- 5.5 cm; p = 0.03) with PSC (32.6 +/- 5.5 cm). A dynamic stretching intervention appears to be more suitable for use as part of a warm-up in young athletes.
Resumo:
O objetivo do presente estudo foi obter informações a respeito do processo de formação de tenistas talentosos e os consequentes fatores que contribuíram para o alcance do profissionalismo. Para tanto, se utilizou uma amostra de cinco tenistas brasileiros profissionais, caracterizados por terem pontos na ATP (Associação dos Tenistas Profissionais). A pesquisa foi constituída através de um delineamento qualitativo, que utilizou como instrumento uma entrevista composta por três perguntas abertas, elaboradas para explorar o contexto de formação do talento. Para a análise dos resultados foi utilizado o “Discurso do Sujeito Coletivo”. De acordo com os resultados, pode-se observar a importância que os professores tiveram no desenvolvimento destes tenistas talentosos, tanto na iniciação como no treinamento. No que se diz respeito ao planejamento dos treinos, o presente estudo demonstra a falta de profissionalismo nas fases iniciais do processo de formação, e a posterior preocupação apenas quando este atleta demonstra possuir algum potencial para seguir carreira profissional.
Resumo:
[ES] El objetivo de este trabajo ha sido determinar el nivel de burnout en un grupo de tenistas de alto rendimiento y conocer la relación existente entre la orientación motivacional y los niveles de burnout, así como la influencia de las variables sociodemográficas.
Resumo:
Nell’ambito della ricerca scientifica nel campo dello sport, la Performance Analysis si sta ritagliando un crescente spazio di interesse. Per Performance Analysis si intende l’analisi della prestazione agonistica sia dal punto di vista biomeccanico che dal punto di vista dell’analisi notazionale. In questa tesi è stata analizzata la prestazione agonistica nel tennistavolo attraverso lo strumento dell’analisi notazionale, partendo dallo studio degli indicatori di prestazione più importanti dal punto di vista tecnico-tattico e dalla loro selezione attraverso uno studio sull’attendibilità nella raccolta dati. L’attenzione è stata posta quindi su un aspetto tecnico originale, il collegamento spostamenti e colpi, ricordando che una buona tecnica di spostamento permette di muoversi rapidamente nella direzione della pallina per effettuare il colpo migliore. Infine, l’obbiettivo principale della tesi è stato quello di confrontare le tre categorie di atleti selezionate: alto livello mondiale maschile (M), alto livello junior europeo (J) ed alto livello mondiale femminile (F). La maggior parte delle azioni cominciano con un servizio corto al centro del tavolo, proseguono con una risposta in push (M) o in flik di rovescio (J). Il colpo che segue è principalmente il top spin di dritto dopo un passo pivot o un top di rovescio senza spostamento. Gli alteti M e J contrattaccano maggiormente con top c. top di dritto e le atlete F prediligono colpi meno spregiudicati, bloccando di rovescio e proseguendo con drive di rovescio. Attraverso lo studio della prestazione di atleti di categorie e generi diversi è possibile migliorare le scelte strategiche prima e durante gli incontri. Le analisi statistiche multivariate (modelli log-lineari) hanno permesso di validare con metodo scientifico sia le procedure già utilizzate in letteratura che quelle innovative messe a punto per la prima volta in occasione di questo studio.
Resumo:
Einleitung und Fragestellung In der Forschung zum Selbstgespräch im Sport dominiert die Frage nach dessen Wir-kung auf die Leistung. Der Einfluss des während der Leistungserbringung geäußerten Selbstgesprächs auf (un-)beteiligte Personen (z.B. Zuschauer, Gegenspieler oder Dop-pelpartner) wurde bislang kaum untersucht. Van Raalte, Brewer, Cornelius und Petitpas (2006) konnten immerhin zeigen, dass Zuschauer einen Tennisspieler als kompetenter einschätzen, wenn er in einem Laborexperiment mit positivem statt mit negativem Selbst-gespräch gezeigt wurde. In unserer Studie stehen Dyaden im Zentrum, in der eine Person eine andere beobach-tet, die mit sich selbst spricht. Untersucht wird, ob bei einer additiven Teamaufgabe sol-che Beobachtungen einen Effekt auf die Kompetenzeinschätzung und die Anstren-gungsbereitschaft haben. Es wird vermutet, dass die beobachtende Person dann eine höhere Anstrengungsbereitschaft (AV) zeigt, wenn sie einen Teampartner mit positivem Selbstgespräch (UV) beobachtet, da sie ihren Partner als kompetenter einschätzt und diese Einschätzung als Mediator wirkt. Methode Es wurde ein randomisierter Zweigruppen-Versuchsplan mit zwei Experimentalgruppen (je n=20) durchgeführt. Das Treatment bestand aus einem Video, das den (jeweils identi-schen) Teampartner bei der Bewältigung der zu absolvierenden visuomotorisch-koordinativen Testaufgabe zeigt (Test 2HAND des Wiener Testsystems). Das Video wur-de abhängig von der Gruppenzugehörigkeit mit negativen respektive positiven Selbstge-sprächsaussagen unterlegt. Die Mediatorvariable wahrgenommene Kompetenz wurde mittels eines Fragebogens erhoben, der in Anlehnung an Van Raalte et al. (2006) entwi-ckelt wurde. Das Vorgehen zur Erhebung der abhängigen Variable Anstrengungsbereit-schaft wurde von Ohlert (2009) übernommen: Die Probanden erhielten die Möglichkeit, sich auf die spätere Testphase vorzubereiten. Erfasst wurde dabei die Zeit, die sie für die Vorbereitung in Anspruch nahmen, und die Genauigkeit, mit der sie eine Reihe von Aufwärmaufgaben absolvierten. Ergebnisse Die Datenerhebung wurde soeben abgeschlossen. Die Ergebnisse der noch ausstehen-den regressionsanalytischen Auswertung werden auf der Tagung präsentiert. Literatur Ohlert, J. (2009). Teamleistung. Social Loafing in der Vorbereitung auf eine Gruppenaufgabe. Hamburg: Dr. Kovac. Van Raalte, J. L., Brewer, B. W., Cornelius, A. E. & Petitpas, A. J. (2006). Self-presentational effects of self-talk on perceptions of tennis players. Hellenic Journal of Psychology, 3, 134-149.
Resumo:
Ausgangslage und Fragestellung In der Forschung zum Selbstgespräch im Sport dominiert die Frage nach dessen Auswir-kungen auf die sprechende Person selber. Gemäss Diaz (1992) besitzt das offene Selbstgespräch aber neben der individuell-regulatorischen auch eine sozial-kommunikative Funktion. Diese sozial-kommunikative Funktion und der damit verbunde-ne Einfluss des Selbstgesprächs auf Beobachter wurden in der Sportwissenschaft bisher nur marginal untersucht (z.B. Gould & Weiss, 1981; Van Raalte, Brewer, Cornelius & Pe-titpas, 2006). Im angestrebten Forschungsvorhaben sollen zwei Fragen geklärt werden: Steht das of-fene, während dem Wettkampf geäusserte Selbstgespräch eines Athleten in Zusammen-hang mit dessen Selbstdarstellung? Beeinflusst das offene Selbstgespräch eines Athle-ten den Dyadenpartner und wie sie sich gegenseitig wahrnehmen? Methode Es werden Doppelpartien im Badminton per Videokamera aufgezeichnet. Mittels einer Korrelationsanalyse soll überprüft werden, ob die so erhobenen Selbstgesprächsaussa-gen mit dem Persönlichkeitsmerkmal „Selbstdarstellung“ der Probanden zusammenhän-gen. Nach Spielschluss werden die Probanden entsprechend eines video-stimulierten Fremdkonfrontationsinterviews mit Spielausschnitten konfrontiert, die offenes Selbstge-spräch ihres Dyadenpartners enthalten. Sie werden dabei nach ihren Kognitionen sowie Emotionen gefragt, die sie während der entsprechenden Spielsituation erlebten. Aktuelle Fragen Welcher Fragebogen zur Erhebung des Persönlichkeitsmerkmals „Selbstdarstellung“ soll Verwendung finden? Zur Auswahl steht ein noch zu übersetzender, validierter, engli-scher und sportspezifischer Fragebogen und die Verwendung eines deutschen Frage-bogens, der habituelle Selbstdarstellungstechniken im Allgemeinen erhebt. Wie soll beim video-stimulierten Fremdkonfrontationsinterview vorgegangen werden? Werden den Probanden z.B. das ganze Spiel oder nur ausgewählte Ausschnitte gezeigt, in denen offenes Selbstgespräch vorkommt? Wie zeitnah muss das Interview durchge-führt werden? Literatur Diaz, R. M. (1992). Methodological Concerns in the Study of Private Speech. In R. M. Diaz & L. E. Berk (Eds.), Private speech. From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 55-81). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Gould, D. & Weiss, M. (1981). The effects of model similarity and model talk on self-efficacy and muscular endurance. Journal of Sport Psychology, 3, 17-29. Van Raalte, J. L., Brewer, B. W., Cornelius, A. E. & Petitpas, A. J. (2006). Self-presentational effects of self-talk on perceptions of tennis players. Hellenic Journal of Psychology, 3, 134-149.
Resumo:
Introduction According to Lent and Lopez’ (2002) tripartite view of efficacy beliefs, members of a team form beliefs about the efficacy of their team partners. This other-efficacy belief can influence individual performance as shown by Dunlop, Beatty, and Beauchamp (2011) in their experimental study using manipulated performance feedback to alter other-efficacy beliefs. Participants holding favorable other-efficacy beliefs outperformed those with lower other--‐efficacy beliefs. Antecedents of such other-efficacy beliefs are amongst others perceptions regarding motivation and psychological factors of the partner (Jackson, Knapp, & Beauchamp, 2008). Overt self-talk could be interpreted as the manifestation of such motivational or psychological factors. In line with this assumption, in an experimental study using dubbed videos of the same segment of a tennis match, Van Raalte, Brewer, Cornelius, and Petitpas (2006) found that players were perceived more favorably (e.g., more concentrated, and of higher ability levels) when shown with dubbed positive self-talk as compared to dubbed negative or no dubbed self--‐talk. Objectives The aim of the study was to examine the possible effects of a confederate’s overt self-talk on participants’ other-efficacy beliefs and performance in a team setting. Method In a laboratory experiment (between-subjects, pre-post-test design, matched by pretest performance) 89 undergraduate students (female = 35, M = 20.81 years, SD = 2.34) participated in a golf putting task together with a confederate (same gender groups). Depending on the experimental condition (positive, negative, or no self-talk), the confederate commented his or her putts according to a self-talk script. Bogus performance feedback assured that the performance of the confederate was held constant. Performance was measured as the distance to the center of the target, other-efficacy by a questionnaire. Results The data collection has just finished and the results of repeated measures analyses of variance will be presented and discussed at the congress. We expect to find higher other-efficacy beliefs and better individual performance in the positive self-talk condition. References Dunlop, W.L., Beatty, D.J., & Beauchamp, M.R. (2011). Examining the influence of other-efficacy and self-efficacy on personal performance. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 33, 586-593. Jackson, B., Knapp, P., & Beauchamp, M.R. (2008). Origins and consequences of tripartite efficacy beliefs within elite athlete dyads. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 30, 512-540. Lent, R.W., & Lopez, F.G. (2002). Cognitive ties that bind: A tripartite view of efficacy beliefs in growth--‐promoting relationships. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 21, 256-286. Van Raalte, J.L., Brewer, B.W, Cornelius, A.E., & Petitpas, A.J. (2006). Self-presentational effects of self-talk on perceptions of tennis players. Hellenic Journal of Psychology, 3, 134-149.
Resumo:
Aunque se han logrado importantes avances en estudios de laboratorio con diseños experimentales poco representativos (e.g., Farrow y Reid, 2012; Nieminen, Piirainen, Salmi, y Linnamo, 2013), a día de hoy, todavía se desconoce a cabalidad cómo los jugadores de tenis de diferente nivel de pericia calibran o ajustan sus movimientos a las demandas espacio-temporales presentes en la tarea de resto de un primer servicio. ! Escasos trabajos se han llevado a cabo in situ y a la mayoría se les puede cuestionar algún aspecto de la metodología empleada. Así pues, en varios estudios la frecuencia de grabación ha sido limitada (e.g., a 50 Hz en Jackson y Gudgeon, 2004; Triolet, Benguigui, Le Runigo y Williams, 2013), o la velocidad del saque ha sido visiblemente inferior a la habitual (cf. Carboch, Süss y Kocib, 2014; Williams, Singer y Weigelt, 1998). También, en algunos estudios los participantes experimentados no han sido jugadores de nivel internacional (e.g., Avilés, Ruiz, Sanz y Navia, 2014), y el tamaño muestral ha sido muy pequeño (e.g., Gillet, Leroy, Thouvarecq, Mégrot y Stein, 2010). ! Además, en los diferentes trabajos se han utilizado una diversidad de métodos e instrumentos de medida y los criterios de codificación del inicio de los movimientos y de las respuestas han diferido; como consecuencia el lapso visomotor de respuesta (LVMr) ha sido muy dispar variando considerablemente de 198 a 410 ms. Considerando los inconvenientes señalados anteriormente, el presente estudio tuvo como objetivo determinar un modelo técnico de regulación temporal de los movimientos y de la respuesta del restador, tomando en cuenta el flujo continuo de información proporcionado por el sacador. Para ello, se realizó un análisis cronométrico de los restos de doce jugadores de diferente nivel deportivo (seis internacionales y seis nacionales) que respondieron de forma natural enviando sus devoluciones hacia las dianas. Se grabaron las acciones de los restadores y sacadores con una cámara Casio Exilim Pro Ex-F1 de alta velocidad (300 Hz) y luego se realizó un análisis imagen por imagen cada 3.33 ms. Una vez obtenidos los datos de los vídeos se realizaron análisis con las pruebas de ANOVA de un factor, ANCOVA con la velocidad del saque como covariable, U de Mann-Whitney y Chi-cuadrado de Pearson. En cuanto a la regulación del movimiento hasta el momento del despegue, los jugadores internacionales iniciaron sus acciones antes que los jugadores nacionales lo que podría indicar una mejor preparación al ejecutar los movimientos como reflejo del nivel de pericia. Los jugadores internacionales iniciaron la elevación del pie posterior a -293 ms y los jugadores nacionales a -202 ms. Todas estas acciones se fueron enlazando unas con otras y fue en el momento del impacto del sacador donde los restadores demostraron una remarcable coordinación perceptivo-motriz. Por consiguiente, los jugadores internacionales despegaron e iniciaron el vuelo a tan solo -6.5 ms del impacto y los jugadores nacionales lo hicieron más tarde a +19.5 ms. A lo largo de la secuencia temporal, todo parece indicar que las informaciones que utilizan los restadores interactúan entre sí; información más temprana y menos fiable para anticipar o moverse antes e información más tardía y más fiable para regular la temporalización de las acciones. Los restadores de nivel internacional y nacional anticiparon a nivel espacial en un bajo porcentaje (7.7% vs. 13.6%) y en tiempos similares (-127 vs. -118 ms) sugiriendo que la utilización de variables ópticas tempranas y menos fiables solo se produce en contadas ocasiones. Por otra parte, estos datos se relacionan con una gran precisión en la respuesta ya que tanto los jugadores internacionales como los nacionales demostraron un alto porcentaje de acierto al responder (95.4% vs. 96.7%). Se había señalado que los jugadores internacionales y nacionales se diferenciarían en el tiempo de caída (i.e., aterrizaje) del primer pie del salto preparatorio, sin embargo ese efecto no fue encontrado (128 vs. 135 ms). Tampoco se hallaron diferencias en el porcentaje de caída con el pie contrario a la dirección de la pelota (58% vs. 62%). Donde sí ambos grupos se diferenciaron fue en el tiempo de caída del segundo pie (147 vs. 168 ms). Esta diferencia de 21 ms fue crucial y fue una prueba de la mayor rapidez de los jugadores internacionales; sugiriendo que ésta acción se podría relacionar con el momento del inicio de la respuesta. Aunque los jugadores internacionales hayan demostrado ser más rápidos en relación con sus capacidades funcionales, ambos grupos no se diferenciaron en todas las variables relacionadas con el LVMr. Ellos no utilizaron esos valiosos milisegundos ganados en el instante de la caída del segundo pie para responder más pronto, ya que el LVMr del miembro superior fue el mismo para ambos grupos (179 vs. 174 ms). Es como si hubiesen tenido todo el tiempo del mundo para seguir ajustando sus acciones hasta el propio golpeo. Además, estos tiempos largos sugieren que en la gran mayoría de los restos la información clave que determinó la respuesta fue detectada (extraída) en momentos cercanos al golpeo del sacador y en la primera parte del vuelo de la pelota. Asimismo, se constató que en general el LVMr se ve influenciado por el tipo de información utilizada. De esta manera, cuando se tomaron en cuenta los ensayos en los que hubo anticipación espacial reflejados en el LVMr del cuerpo entero los tiempos disminuyeron (152 vs. 136 ms). Por otra parte, existieron ocasiones (13%) en los que tanto los jugadores internacionales como los nacionales respondieron tarde recibiendo saques directos (208 vs. 195 ms). Es muy posible que en estos casos los jugadores hayan tenido problemas para detectar la información respondiendo fuera de los márgenes temporales de acción lo que mermó su rendimiento. Lo mismo pudo haber ocurrido cuando ambos grupos de jugadores corrigieron el movimiento del miembro superior tras el impacto (17% vs. 10%) lo que aumentó el tiempo en responder al redirigir la respuesta hacia el lado correcto (208 vs. 205 ms). Además, los jugadores internacionales obtuvieron tiempos de movimiento menores que el de los jugadores nacionales (509 vs. 531 ms) lo que se reflejó en un tiempo total de actuación menor (683 vs. 703 ms). Por último, en cuanto al rendimiento del resto, los jugadores internacionales obtuvieron valores superiores a los jugadores nacionales (1.3 vs. 0.9). ABSTRACT Although there have been significant advances in laboratory studies with unrepresentative experimental designs (e.g., Farrow y Reid, 2012; Nieminen, Piirainen, Salmi, y Linnamo, 2013), today it is still unknown to full extent how tennis players of different levels of expertise calibrate or adjust their movements to the spatial-temporal demands present in the return of a first serve. Few studies have been carried out in situ and some aspects of the methodology most of them used can be questioned. Thus, in several studies the recording frequency has been limited (e.g., a 50 Hz en Jackson y Gudgeon, 2004; Triolet, Benguigui, Le Runigo y Williams, 2013), or serve speed was visibly lower than the usual one (cf. Carboch, Süss y Kocib, 2014; Williams, Singer y Weigelt, 1998). Also, in some studies, experienced participants have not played at international level (e.g., Avilés, Ruiz, Sanz y Navia, 2014), and the sample size has been very small (e.g., Gillet, Leroy, Thouvarecq, Mégrot y Stein, 2010). Furthermore, different works have used a variety of methods and measurement instruments and coding criteria of the onset of movements and responses have differed; due to this, visuomotor response delay (LVMr) has been very uneven, varying considerably from 198-410 ms. Considering the drawbacks mentioned above, this study aimed to determine a technical model of temporal regulation of movements and returner’s response, taking into account the continuous flow of information provided by the server. For this, a chronometric analysis of the returns of twelve players (six international and six national) of different sports level, that naturally responded by hitting their returns towards the targets, was performed. Actions of servers and returners were recorded with a Casio Exilim Pro Ex-F1 high speed camera (300 Hz) and then every 3.33 ms analysis was made frame by frame. Once the data of the videos were obtained, analyses were performed using one factor ANOVA test, ANCOVA with the speed of the serve as a covariate, U of Mann- Whitney and Pearson’s Chi-square test. As for the regulation of movement until the moment of serve, international players began their actions before national players, which could indicate that they were better prepared to execute movements reflecting the level of their expertise. International players began raising the rear foot at -293 ms and national players at -202 ms. All these actions were being linked to each other and it was at the moment of impact of the server when the receivers demonstrated a remarkable perceptual-motor coordination. Therefore, international players took off and started their flight just -6.5 ms before the serve and national players did the same somewhat later: +19.5 ms after the serve. Along the timeline, everything seems to indicate that the information used by returners interact with each other; early information which is less reliable to anticipate or move before, and later information more reliable appears to regulate the timing of actions. Returners of international and national levels anticipated at spatial level in a low percentage (7.7% vs. 13.6%) and in similar times (-127 vs. -118 ms) suggesting that the use of early and less reliable optical variables is only produced on rare occasions. Moreover, these data relate to a precise response as both international and national players showed a high percentage of success in responding (95.4% vs. 96.7%). It had been noted that international and national players would differ in the time the fall (i.e., landing) of the first foot of the split-step, however, this effect was not found (128 vs. 135 ms). No differences in the percentage of fall with the opposite foot to the direction of the ball (58% vs. 62%) were found. Where the two groups differed was in the time of the fall of the second foot (147 vs. 168 ms). This difference of 21 ms was crucial and it was a proof of mayor speed of international players; suggesting that this action could be related to the onset time of response. Although international players have proven to be faster in relation to their functional capabilities, both groups did not differ in all variables related to LVMr. They did not use those precious milliseconds earned at the time of the fall of the second foot to respond as soon, since the LVMr of the upper limb was the same for both groups (179 vs. 174 ms). It is as if they had all the time in the world to continue to adjust their actions until the return itself. Furthermore, these long times suggest that in the vast majority of the returns, key information that determined the response was detected (pick-up) in moments close to the hit of the server and in the first part of the ball flight. It was also found that in general the LVMr is influenced by the type of information used. Thus, when taking into account the trials during which there was spatial anticipation, reflected in LVMr of the whole body, the times decreased (152 vs. 136 ms). On the other hand, there were occasions (13%) where both international and national players responded late, thus receiving aces (208 vs. 195 ms). It is quite possible that in these cases the players have had trouble to pick-up information, responding out of temporary margins of action, which affected their performance. The same could have occurred when both groups of players corrected upper limb movement after impact (17% vs. 10%), which increased the time to respond and to redirect the return towards the right side (208 vs. 205 ms). Moreover, international players scored lower movement times than the national players (509 vs. 531 ms), which was reflected in a shorter total response time (683 vs. 703 ms). Finally, as far as the performance of return is concerned, international players scored above the national players values (1.3 vs. 0.9).
Resumo:
Galvao, D.A., and D.R. Taaffe. Single- vs. multiple-set resistance training: recent developments in the controversy. J. Strength Cond. Res. 18(3):660-667. 2004.-The number of sets in a resistance training program remains a major point of discussion and controversy. Studies prior to 1998 demonstrated inconsistent findings between single-set and multiple-set programs; however, recent evidence suggests that multiple sets promote additional benefits following short- and long-term training. The rationale supporting multiple sets is that the number of sets is part of the exercise volume equation, and the volume of exercise is crucial in producing the stimulus necessary to elicit specific physiological adaptations. The purpose of this paper is to present an overview of recent resistance training studies comparing single and multiple sets. However, it should be noted that studies to date have been conducted in young and middle-aged adults, and it remains to be determined if the additional benefits accrued with multiple-set training also occurs for older adults, especially the frail elderly.
Resumo:
Two experiments were conducted to examine whether the conclusions drawn regarding the timing of anticipatory information pick-up from temporal occlusion studies are influenced by whether (a) the viewing period is of variable or fixed duration and (b) the task is a laboratory-based one with simple responses or a natural one requiring a coupled, interceptive movement response. Skilled and novice tennis players either made pencil-and-paper predictions of service direction (Experiment 1) or attempted to hit return strokes (Experiment 2) to tennis serves while their vision was temporally occluded in either a traditional progressive mode (where more information was revealed in each subsequent occlusion condition) or a moving window mode (where the visual display was only available for a fixed duration with this window shifted to different phases of the service action). Conclusions regarding the timing of information pick-up were generally consistent across display mode and across task setting lending support to the veracity and generalisability of findings regarding perceptual expertise in existing laboratory-based progressive temporal occlusion studies.
Resumo:
Resistance training has been shown to reliably and substantially enhance muscle function in older adults and these improvements can be accompanied by improved functional performance. Training variables should be manipulated to enhance muscle strength and minimize injury risks in this population.
Resumo:
PURPOSE: We aimed to a) introduce a new Test to Exhaustion Specific to Tennis (TEST) and compare performance (test duration) and physiological responses to those obtained during the 20-m multistage shuttle test (MSST), and b) determine to which extent those variables correlate with performance level (tennis competitive ranking) for both test procedures. METHODS: Twenty-seven junior players (8 males, 19 females) members of the national teams of the French Tennis Federation completed MSST and TEST, including elements of the game (ball hitting, intermittent activity, lateral displacement), in a randomized order. Cardiorespiratory responses were compared at submaximal (respiratory compensation point) and maximal loads between the two tests. RESULTS: At the respiratory compensation point oxygen uptake (50.1 +/- 4.7 vs. 47.5 +/- 4.3 mL.min-1.kg-1, p = 0.02), but not minute ventilation and heart rate, was higher for TEST compared to MSST. However, load increment and physiological responses at exhaustion did not differ between the two tests. Players' ranking correlated negatively with oxygen uptake measured at submaximal and maximal loads for both TEST (r = -0.41; p = 0.01 and -0.55; p = 0.004) and MSST (r = -0.38; P = 0.05 and -0.51; p = 0.1). CONCLUSION: Using TEST provides a tennis-specific assessment of aerobic fitness and may be used to prescribe aerobic exercise in a context more appropriate to the game than MSST. Results also indicate that VO2 values both at submaximal and maximal load reached during TEST and MSST are moderate predictors of players competitive ranking.
Resumo:
The physiological and perceptual demands together with match notation of a four-set tennis match were studied in two elite professional players during the preparation for the 2008 Davis Cup. The design of this case report is unique in that it is the first to describe the demands of prolonged match-play (197 min) over four sets in ecologically valid conditions. The variables measured before and after each set included blood lactate and glucose concentrations, body mass, and perception of effort. Stroke count for each rally and heart rate were recorded during each set while salivary cortisol concentration was determined before and after the match. The rally length decreased as the match progressed. The results showed significant physiological stress, with each player losing greater than 2.5% of body mass (as fluid) and having elevated salivary cortisol concentrations after the match. Heart rate and perception of effort were also increased following each set indicating increasing stress. However, blood lactate decreased following the fourth set while blood glucose was maintained. The results also suggest that elite players may adjust work rates or tactics to cope with the increased perception of effort. This report shows that four sets of tennis are associated with increasing stress and fatigue.
Resumo:
To examine the time course of alteration in neural process (spinal loop properties) during prolonged tennis playing, 12 competitive players performed a series of neuromuscular tests every 30 min during a 3-h match protocol. Muscle activation (twitch interpolation) and normalized EMG activity were assessed during maximal voluntary contraction (MVC) of plantar flexors. Spinal reflexes and M-waves were evoked at rest (i.e., H(max) and M(max) , respectively) and during MVC (i.e., H(sup) , V-wave, M(sup) , respectively). MVC torque declined significantly (P<0.001) across the match protocol, due to decrease (P<0.001) in muscle activation and in normalized EMG activity. The impairment in MVC was significantly correlated (r=0.77; P<0.05) with the decline in muscle activation. H(max) /M(max) (P<0.001), H(sup) /M(sup) (P<0.01) and V/M(sup) (P<0.05) ratios were depressed with fatigue and decreased by ∼80%, 46% and 61% at the end of exercise, respectively. Simultaneously, peak twitch torque and M-wave amplitude were significantly (P<0.01) altered with exercise, suggesting peripheral alterations. During prolonged tennis playing, the compromised voluntary strength capacity is linked to a reduced neural input to the working muscles. This central activation deficit partly results from a modulation in spinal loop properties.
Resumo:
L’objectiu d’aquest estudi és determinar la zona més efectiva per guanyar el punt mitjançant l’atac de dreta en el tennis de taula. S’utilitza una adaptació del sistema d’anàlisi dels tres nivells d’habilitat, proposat pels acadèmics xinesos Wu i Li (1990) per tal de classificar els punts de la forma que ens permetin analitzar les zones de finalització i les formes com s’obtenen els punts. L’estudi analitza cinc partits dels sis millors jugadors del rànquing mundial de la ITTF. El resultat de l’estudi és que la zona de finalització més eficaç per obtenir el punt mitjançant l’atac de dreta és la zona 1 de la taula del rival.