214 resultados para degenerate primers
Resumo:
We investigate within mean-field theory the influence of a one-dimensional optical lattice and of trapped degenerate fermions on the critical rotational frequency for vortex line creation in a Bose-Einstein condensate. We consider laser intensities of the lattice such that quantum coherence across the condensate is ensured. We find a sizable decrease of the thermodynamic critical frequency for vortex nucleation with increasing applied laser strength and suggest suitable parameters for experimental observation. Since 87Rb-40K mixtures may undergo collapse, we analyze the related question of how the optical lattice affects the mechanical stability of the system.
Resumo:
Distortions of polyacene polymers are studied within a many-body valence-bond framework using a powerful transfer-matrix technique for the valence-bond (or Heisenberg) model of the system. The computations suggest that the ground-state geometry is either totally symmetric or possibly exhibits a slight (A2 or B2 symmetry) bond-alternation distortion. The lowest-energy (nonsymmetric, in-plane) distortions are the A2 and B2 modes, which, within our approximations, are degenerate.
Resumo:
The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.
Resumo:
La reducció de l'interès del 5 % al 3 % ha estat considerada el principi de la fi dels censals. Però aquest argument no té en compte que la de 1750 va ser només l'última d'una sèrie de reduccions del for de censal, que també es produïren a Anglaterra, França o Castella, i que els primers símptomes de retracció de l'oferta de préstecs censals no apareixerien fins a la darreria del segle XVIII o principi del segle XIX. L¿article proposa una explicació alternativa d¿aquelles reduccions, que ajustaven l'interès a l'alça coetània del valor patrimonial de la terra i la minva de la rendibilitat del seu esmerç, i de la fi del censal a partir de l'impagament de pensions que es desencadenà a les acaballes del segle XVIII, juntament amb la vaga de delmes i rendes de la terra, quan s'havien esgotat, en bona part, les possibilitats d'expansió agrària del segle XVIII.
Resumo:
We have studied the structural changes that fatty acid monolayers in the Ov phase undergo when a simple shear flow is imposed. A strong coupling is revealed by the changes in domain structure that are observable using Brewster angle microscopy, suggesting the possibility of shear alignment. The dependence of the alignment on the molecular polar tilt proves that the mechanism is different than in nematic liquid crystals. We argue that the degenerate lattice symmetry lines of the underlying pseudohexagonal lattice align in the flow direction, and we explain the observed alignment angle using geometrical arguments.
Resumo:
Aquest llibre del doctor Corbella és una història de la toxicologia en les seves dues vessants: una història dels verins i altres substàncies tòxiques i, alhora, una història de com s'ha enfocat i amb quins mitjans l'estudi d'aquestes substàncies al llarg de la història des dels primers testimonis escrits.Ja pel que fa a èpoques més modernes, el llibre presta atenció a altres fenòmens com les drogodependències o els efectes tòxics, dits secundaris, de fàrmacs legals.
Resumo:
Els estudis sobre territoris iberics -no sobre arees territorials més o menys coherents, com l'estudi classic de Llobregat (1972) sobre la Contestania o el de Lillo (1981) sobre l'area murciana, que són una altra COsa- compten ja amb una tradició relativament llarga, des deis primers treballs de F. Burillo (1982) sobre la val! mitjana de l'Ebre i els d' A. Ruiz i M. Molinos (1984) al' Alt Guadalquivir, posteriorment els de J. Bernabeu, H. Bonet, P. Guerin i C. Mata a l'Edetania (en particular, Bonet, 1995) i els que diferents autors han realitzat a les arees costaneres de Catalunya (Martín i Plana, 2001). El trebal! d'Ignacio Grau Mira s'insereix plenament dins d'aquesta tradició i, com és lógic, se'n beneficia en gran mesura.
Resumo:
En aquest treball donem a coneixer els primers resultats sobre la flora algologica del Pais Valencia. El material que hem pogut recollir i determinar fins aquest moment procedeix de setze localitats de la costa que estem estudiant, compresa entre Peniscola i el Cap de Santa Pola.
Resumo:
El diposit consisteix en dos filons E-W, tallats per un altre N-S. El rebliment filonia ès zonat amb esfalerita- galena-calcita als nivells profunds i galena-baritina als superiors. Els elements traça en esfalerites i galenes, plenament separades, han estat analitzats mitjancant fluorescència de raigs X. Els resultats mostren que l'esfalerita concentra la majoria d'elements traça, preferentment Fe, Co, Cd, Sn, Sb, Ga, Cu, Ge i Ag, amb una bona correlació entre els sis primers. La galena concentra únicament Sb, Ag i As, amb bona correlació entre els dos primers. Els continguts en elements traça estan homogèniament distribuits en tot el diposit.
Resumo:
L'avançament de l'edat d'aprenentatge obligatori d'una llengua estrangera deis 11 als 8 anys en el context educatiu espanyol, així com la demanda creixent per part deis pares d'introduir-ne l'ensenyament al més aviat possible, ja sigui al Primer Cicle de Primaria (6-7 anys) o encara més aviat, en l'Educació Infantil (3-5 anys), no solament indica un alt nivell de conscienciació de la necessitat de coneixement d'idiomes que el món actual exigeix, sinó que també evidencia la creença que existeix una edat privilegiada per aprendre Ilengües, que se situa en els primers anys de l'escolaritat.
Resumo:
The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.
Resumo:
Els almogàvers foren un grup de guerrers mercenaris que se singularitzava en el si dels exèrcits de la Corona d'Aragó. Esmentats per Jaume el Conqueridor i descrits per Desclot, són els protagonistes dels capítols en què Ramon Muntaner narra la seva màxima gesta: l'aventura en terres bizantines, a conseqüència de la qual els reis d'Aragó seran ducs d'Atenes i Neopàtria. Posteriorment el seu exemple serà al·legat en temps de dificultat Guerra de Successi, Guerra contra Napoleó fins que la Renaixença els reivindicarà i en farà un mite. El present article estudia com els tractà la literatura catalana, i també la castellana, des dels primers textos i sobretot durant els segles XIX i XX, en un recorregut en què es revela com un mite imperfecte, car els aspectes violents remarcats per la propaganda hel·lènica han estat un llast impossible d'eliminar.
Les creences populars entorn a l'aprenentatge d'una llengua estrangera (Anglès) abans dels vuit anys
Resumo:
L'avançament de l'edat d'aprenentatge obligatori d'una llengua estrangera deis 11 als 8 anys en el context educatiu espanyol, així com la demanda creixent per part deis pares d'introduir-ne l'ensenyament al més aviat possible, ja sigui al Primer Cicle de Primaria (6-7 anys) o encara més aviat, en l'Educació Infantil (3-5 anys), no solament indica un alt nivell de conscienciació de la necessitat de coneixement d'idiomes que el món actual exigeix, sinó que també evidencia la creença que existeix una edat privilegiada per aprendre llengües, que se situa en els primers anys de l'escolaritat.
Resumo:
L'article analitza els primers autors (Pere Miquel Carbonell, Ermolao Barbaro, Jeroni Pau) i les primeres obres que citen inscripcions valencianes d'època romana. Tot seguit es posa en relació la informació present en aquestes obres amb la tradició de l'Antiquus i de l'Antiquissimus. Es conclou que el coneixement sobre l'epigrafia valenciana en el primer Renaixement entre els cercles humanístics italians i catalans és molt més ric i més antic del que tradicionalment es creia.
Resumo:
Fins on hem pogut esbrinar, el primer estudi publicat en una revista científica sobre la praderia o alguer de Posidonia oceanica de les illes Medes és el de De Haro (1965). En aquella època, la investigació delsfons marins estava als seus inicis i probablement la praderia de les illes Medes va ser un dels primers llocs en què, amb mitjans força precaris i a càrrec d’uns joves estudiants, es van efectuar prospeccions en immersió, tècnica que tot just començava però que molt ràpidament havia de substituir les dragues ialtres artefactes menys fiables. Amb el Programa de Bentos, entre 1972-1974 (Ros, 1982) –precisament aquest autor (vegeu el capítol 1) era un d’aquells «joves estudiants»; l’altre era en Jordi Camp, actualment investigador del CSIC–, la recerca bentònica va agafar un primer impuls i l’escafandre autònom es va incorporar plenament com a eina d’observació i mostreig. Durant aquest programa, es va situar una de les estacions de treball a les illes Medes, i l’alguer de Posidonia va ser un dels hàbitats objecte d’estudi, tot i que més aviat amb un objectiu faunístic i biocenòtic que estrictament ecològic.