993 resultados para Cervantes Saavedra, Miguel de, 1547-1616. Don Quijote de la Mancha-Crítica i interpretació
Resumo:
The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.
Resumo:
El artculo pasa sucintamente revista a las principales teoras sobre la naturaleza dela mente, desde la Antigedad clsica hasta nuestros das, y toma partido por una concepcin monista materialista que, sin embargo, preserva la especificidad de la mente (el yo) como nudo relaciona1 y centro irreductible de perspectiva que, precisamente por eso, no puede nunca adquirir una visin objetiva de s mismo como puro sujeto.
Resumo:
Con la finalidad de adaptarnos al EEES, desarrollamos una herramienta que nos permitiera realizar un proceso de evaluacin continua de la asignatura troncal de Toxicologa. En el presente trabajo presentamos los resultados de este modelo en el que utilizamos los seminarios como elementos bsicos de este proceso. Describimos cmo se estructuran y desarrollan estos seminarios, as como el modelo de evaluacin de los mismos. Los seminarios fueron evaluados con una puntuacin mxima del 30 % sobre la nota final de la signatura, y la participacin en los mismos con un mximo del 10 %. Algunos de estos seminarios incorporaban evaluaciones realizadas antes del desarrollo de los mismos, que denominbamos pre, y otras justo al final del desarrollo de los mismos, que denominbamos post. Esta herramienta de evaluacin continua se ha mostrado muy eficaz en lo que respecta al grado de participacin y preparacin de los alumnos. Adems, ha supuesto un cambio significativo en el grado de implicacin de los profesores, y una mejora de la comunicacin alumno-profesor.
Resumo:
Con la finalidad de adaptarnos al EEES, desarrollamos una herramienta que nos permitiera realizar un proceso de evaluacin continua de la asignatura troncal de Toxicologa. En el presente trabajo presentamos los resultados de este modelo en el que utilizamos los seminarios como elementos bsicos de este proceso. Describimos cmo se estructuran y desarrollan estos seminarios, as como el modelo de evaluacin de los mismos. Los seminarios fueron evaluados con una puntuacin mxima del 30 % sobre la nota final de la signatura, y la participacin en los mismos con un mximo del 10 %. Algunos de estos seminarios incorporaban evaluaciones realizadas antes del desarrollo de los mismos, que denominbamos pre, y otras justo al final del desarrollo de los mismos, que denominbamos post. Esta herramienta de evaluacin continua se ha mostrado muy eficaz en lo que respecta al grado de participacin y preparacin de los alumnos. Adems, ha supuesto un cambio significativo en el grado de implicacin de los profesores, y una mejora de la comunicacin alumno-profesor.
Resumo:
La presencia de calcita como fase secundaria en cermicas arqueolgicas ha sido citada en diversos trabajos. Sin embargo, su identificacin e interpretacin han sido siempre problemticas. La calcita secundaria se presenta bajo diferentes hbitos que deben reconocerse por microscopia ptica de luz transmitida o por microscopia electrnica de barrido. Ms problemtica resulta su interpretacin segn se trate de un aporte completamente alctono, parcialmente alctono o la concurrencia de ambos factores. En el presente trabajo se exponen dos ejemplos en los que se identifica calcita secundaria, pero en los que es interpretada de manera diferente. La interpretacin depende de cada caso de estudio y requiere el concurso de diversas tcnicas.
Resumo:
The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.
Resumo:
La situacin actual de la prevencin del delito es una consecuencia de un conjunto de cambios socioculturales y de una profundizacin en las tcnicas y mtodos evaluativos de la realidad social del crimen y su impacto social. Diversas teoras se han ido elaborando como sistemas explicativos del mismo. Tras analizarse los mbitos y niveles de prevencin se destacan algunos programas. El artculo se adentra en una revisin crtica de la educacin en prevencin del delito recogiendo algunos ejemplos. Finalmente, se describe de forma precisa una experiencia educativa integral a nivel europeo actualmente en fase de consolidacin.
Resumo:
Introducci: Les persones ostomitzades presenten una srie de canvis de tipus fisiolgic, psicolgic i social, els quals els obliga a adaptar-se a la seva vida diria. Per adaptar-se a aquesta nova situaci utilitzen una srie d'estratgies d'afrontament, sent l'acceptaci el producte final. Per a que les estratgies adoptades siguin les ms efectives, han de rebre una educaci sanitria adequada.Contrriament, l'escassetat d'educaci sanitria o l'educaci sanitria inadequada que reben aquests pacients s un problema real que ens trobem en l'actualitat.Objectiu: Conixer si l'educaci sanitria rebuda pels pacients ostomitzats, t influencia en la seva adaptaci a la nova situaci produda per l'ostomia digestiva.Metodologia: Estudi qualitatiu fenomenolgic, on les dades es recolliran a travs d'entrevistessemiestructurades a pacients amb ostomia digestiva, els quals van rebre l'alta hospitalria fa almenys 2 mesos. Les entrevistes seran gravades, transcrites i analitzades, utilitzant en aquest ltim pas la triangulaci d'investigadors. La mostra ser de tipus intencional, la seleccionar a l'Hospital Universitri de Bellvitge entre els pacients que acudeixen a la consulta de la infermera estomaterapeuta, i el seu tamany estar definit per la saturaci terica. L'estudi estar reglat en tot moment pel dret a la intimitat, incloent en aquest l'anonimat i la confidencialitat, i el dret a la lliure decisi.Consideracions finals: Inclou una explicaci argumentada sobre els meus punts febles i forts del treball, una reflexi sobre la satisfacci amb les competncies que he adquirit i finalment una autoavaluaci dels resultats del meu aprenentatge.
Resumo:
Lobjectiu daquest article s presentar dues situacions diferents que es van produir a la Comunitat de Preveres de Sallent durant la segona meitat del segle XVIII i que permeten analitzar alguns aspectes del funcionament de les comunitats de preveres en general. En primer lloc, un conflicte intern produt per la prdua de rendes desprs de la reducci de 1750. En segon lloc, els problemes que es van generar entre la Comunitat i la poblaci a causa dunes prctiques indignes dalguns dels membres de la Comunitat. Finalment, els llevadors de comptes de March Sanmart, que va ser collector entre 1775 i 1778, a partir dels quals analitzem les diferents procedncies dels ingressos. Lelecci dels temes ha estat donada per la mateixa documentaci. La parrquia de Sallent va perdre larxiu durant la Guerra Civil i, per tant, hem utilitzat el fons de lArxiu Episcopal de Vic i el fons de larxiu del mas Sanmart, don era originari el collector.
Resumo:
Es tracta duna obra fruit dun conveni de collaboraci signat lany 2004 entre la URV, lICAC i Repsol YPF, i resultat dun projecte de recerca de lICAC incls en la lnia de recerca Arqueologia del paisatge, poblament i territori, que ha comptat amb el cofinanament de Repsol YPF i la collaboraci de Codex. Aquesta obra se centra en les excavacions arqueolgiques efectuades en una villa romana situada entre la via Tarraco-Ilerda i la llera del riu Francol, a lactual terme municipal del Morell. s un assentament datat entre els segles II aC i V dC que reflecteix els patrons docupaci romana a lrea central de lager Tarraconensis i complementa, a partir duna exhaustiva anlisi, les dades histriques que, fins a lactualitat, oferien les villes de Centcelles a Constant o de Paret Delgada a la Selva del Camp. Lestudi parteix de les excavacions que lempresa Codex hi va efectuar lany 1996 arran duna actuaci durgncia motivada per la construcci duna nova planta industrial de laleshores Repsol Qumica SA. Lobra s fruit de lestudi de la documentaci, fins ara indita, elaborada per lempresa Codex i que ha estat cedida per a aquest projecte de recerca. La publicaci ha estat coordinada per Josep M. Macias Sol i Joan J. Menchon Bes i els textos sn obra del Dr. Domnec Campillo, M. Milagros Cuesta i Laura Devenat (Laboratori de Paleopatologia i Paleoantropologia, Museu dArqueologia de Catalunya), Montserrat Garca Noguera (Codex Arqueologia i Patrimoni), Miguel . Gonzlez Prez (Grup de Recerca dArqueologia Clssica, Protohistrica i Egpcia, Universitat de Barcelona), Josep M. Macias Sol (ICAC), Joan J. Menchon Bes (Museu dHistria de Tarragona), Joan S. Mestres i Torres (Laboratori de Dataci per Radiocarboni, Universitat de Barcelona), Rosario Navarro Sez (Universitat de Barcelona), Josep M. Palet Martnez (ICAC) i Jordi Principal Ponce (Museu dArqueologia de Catalunya). Amb la nova collecci Hic et nunc lICAC assumeix el comproms de difondre regularment la documentaci arqueolgica bsica generada des del camp universitari o empresarial, entenent que la publicaci daspectes ms concrets s imprescindible per a lelaboraci de sntesis ms globalitzadores.
Resumo:
La guerra de Laos es probablemente uno de los episodios de la Guerra Fra ms desconocidos e ignorados por la historiografa a pesar de las terribles consecuencias que tuvo. El hecho de que frecuentemente se considere como un conflicto perifrico y ligado a la Guerra de Vietnam no contribuye tampoco a mejorar esta imagen.
Resumo:
The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.
Resumo:
The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.
Resumo:
Aquest treball t per objectiu mostrar que el joc constitueix un dels trets inherents de la cultura universal i, per extensi, de la tradici occidental. Naturalment, el joc ha estat objecte de diverses consideracions morals, lligades en ms duna ocasi a la religi. En alguns moments i tamb des del cristianisme shan condemnat amb rigor i severitat les prctiques ldiques i, molt especialment, els jocs datzar i el mn dels espectacles. De fet, la realitat histrica aqu analitzada a travs del cas concret del jeu de paume confirma lexistncia duna activitat ldica ben significativa que, malgrat totes les prevencions, va ser tolerada i fins i tot aprovada pels humanistes del Renaixement en acceptar la seva vlua educativa. Sortosament, aquells savis humanistes comptaven amb leutraplia, una virtut darrel aristotlica que va ser cristianitzada per sant Toms dAquino i que, com es veur a continuaci, tamb va arribar a casa nostra a la Baixa Edat Mitjana a travs de Francesc Eiximenis. Si alg pensa que la gent dabans no es divertia, comet un gran error, perqu el nostre passat histric s curull de jocs i activitats ldiques. Nogensmenys, leutraplia tamb es perfila com una virtut social, tal com apareix en diferents autors de lEdat Moderna, com ara Cervantes, Gracin i Feijoo.
Resumo:
[Traditions. Asie. Inde]