999 resultados para Persones grans dependents
Resumo:
Proposit: L'objectiu d'aquest treball és analitzar quins efectes produeix un programa d'activitat física en pacients amb trastorn bipolar de tipus II. Mètodes: Per la realització d'aquest estudi he comptat amb una mostra petita (n=14), la qual estava dividida en grup control (n=7) i grup intervenció (n=7). Es va realitzar una valoració inicial de la mostra per mitjà del Yesavage i SF-12. Posteriorment el grup intervenció va participar en 20 sessions d'activitat física (freq: 2/set). Un cop finalitzat el programa es va tornar a valorar la mostra per mitjà del Yesavage i SF-12. Resultats: Els subjectes del grup intervenció van millorar en: Estat d'ànim (Yesavage), Percepció de la pròpia salut, nivell d'energia i sentiment de tristesa i desànim. En l'estat de tranquil·litat i calma no van mostrar una gran variació. Conclusions: La pràctica d'activitat física en grup millora l'estat d'ànim, percepció de pròpia salut, redueix l'estat de tristesa i desànim i nivell d'energia.
Resumo:
En aquest article dedicat a Norbert Elias en commemoració dels vint anys de la seva mort, proposem revisar, de manera resumida, la seva obra a partir dels interrogants ontològics, epistemològics i metodològics que Elias va encarar al llarg de la seva dilatada vida acadèmica i de recerca. L’article pretén ser, alhora, un punt i a part en el monogràfic, perquè en vol recollir alguns dels interrogants, debats i conclusions, com també tenir un paper introductori per a aquelles persones que, sense gaires coneixements previs sobre l’autor de Breslau, vulguin conèixer alguns dels trets definitoris de la seva trajectòria intel·lectual al llarg del segle XX. I, sobretot, quin llegat, en forma de grans preguntes, deixa per a la sociologia del segle XXI. L’article acaba amb una bibliografia escollida d’obres clau.
Resumo:
L’escola és un reflex de la societat en la que s’inscriu. Una societat democràtica comporta una escola que visqui i practiqui la democràcia. Els nostres drets, les nostres llibertats i els nostres deures descansen sobre la nostra autonomia i responsabilitat. Una manera de treballar aquests aspectes a l’escola és des del projecte de convivència de centre, on s’ha d’incardinar tota la vida del centre educatiu. Educar en valors exigeix conviure amb ells en tots els àmbits de la nostra vida. Educar en valors significa, també, educar la capacitat de valorar. Aquesta capacitat de valorar ens permet ser persones lliures, crítiques, justes i solidàries, conscients de la realitat del nostre entorn més immediat i del context mundial en general, i compromeses amb la humanitat, amb les generacions futures i amb la sostenibilitat del planeta. D’acord amb aquestes consideracions, el projecte de convivència de centre pot fonamentar-se en els següents tres grans blocs de valors, en els quals hi podem encabir els valors, entesos com les nostres opcions de vida: a) el valor de la persona humana i la seva dignitat; b) el valor de la democràcia i la ciutadania; i c) el valor de la igualtat i la diferència.
Resumo:
Es descriu l’evolució del poblament des de l’època preibèrica fins a l’ibèric, tot seguint J. Sanmartí, que l’interpreta com a resultat d’una dinàmica interna de la societat que va conduir a la formació de protoestats o estats arcaics en el període ibèric ple. A partir de l’arribada dels romans, el 218 aC, l’àrea catalana va sofrir el canvi de poblament més radical que ha experimentat el país des del neolític, a conseqüència de la introducció del sistema de la ciutat romana. Això es deu al pas d’una societat amb una estructura que es podria interpretar com un protoestat o estat arcaic a una societat que correspon a un estat de dret, el de la república romana. Es fa molt difícil fer càlculs sobre les xifres de poblament, tot i que se suposa que la població va augmentar en època romana. El canvi radica en el pas d’una densa xarxa jerarquitzada de poblats encastellats, dalt de turons, i amb alguns establiments rurals dispersos pel territori, a l’establiment de les ciutats romanes, que buscaven les millors planes per fer-hi una producció agrícola que va entrar en el comerç de llargues distàncies del Mediterrani de l’època. Les ciutats romanes es troben en l’origen de les principals ciutats de la Catalunya actual.
Resumo:
Sóc de l’opinió que hem d’analitzar acuradament els fets històrics precedents i veure què feien els nostres avantpassats per conèixer i viure més intensament el present, i també per encarar amb més precisió el futur. Saber en quines situacions es van trobar ens pot servir per plantar cara al present, amb actituds, coneixements i intuïcions que provenen de l’herència de la història que ens precedeix.Ben segur que és aquest el principal motiu que m'ha empès a dur a terme aquesta recerca i convertir-la en el meu Treball de Fi de Grau. Cal analitzar l’exili republicà, conseqüència directa de la victòria de l’exèrcit rebel. Amb l'inici de la dictadura de Francisco Franco, el procés demogràfic generacional a Espanya va patir un greu sotrac. Amb la “modernització del país” propugnada pel Caudillo, molts homes i dones es van veure forçats a marxar precipitadament del seu poble, d'on provenien i on tenien les seves arrels, per arribar a grans ciutats, totalment contràries a la seva manera de viure rural. Aquests milions de persones que van deixar el país o van canviar de domicili van ser víctimes d'aquest “nou ordre” franquista.
Resumo:
El càncer de colon és un problema mundial, amb una incidència anual d’1 milió de casos i una mortalitat anual de més de 500.000. A causa de l’envelliment i la expansió de les poblacions el nombre absolut de casos augmentarà en les pròximes dos dècades tan en els països desenvolupats com en els no desenvolupats. D’acord amb la Societat Estado Unidense d’ Oncologia (American Cancer Society) el càncer de Colon és una de les causes més importants de mort relacionades amb càncer als Estats Units. Es desconeixen les causes de l’origen però no els factors que influeixen a desencadenar aquest tipus de càncer per això és important saber quins són i com poder evitar-los. És un dels càncers més predominants i mortals d’avui dia, per tant la societat hauria d’intentar minimitzar al màxim l’exposició a aquests factors que comporten a desenvolupar el càncer. També es desconeix el risc de presentar-lo i per tan existeix una important falta d’informació. El càncer es pot prevenir i curar per tan es possible identificar als individus amb major risc a presentar-lo. Els coneixements, l’ansietat i la percepció del risc poden interferir en l’acceptació de programes preventius.
Resumo:
The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.
Resumo:
The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.
Resumo:
Aquest treball es proposa analitzar la traducció del doblatge i la subtitulació d'un text audiovisual concret per identificar les tendències i les estratègies de traducció que imperen en cada modalitat. Així mateix, pretén comprovar si a la pràctica es respecten les convencions i normes de la traducció audiovisual
Resumo:
L'1 de setembre va morir a Barcelona el primer catedràtic de genètica de l'estat, el professor Antoni Prevosti i Pelegrín.
Resumo:
Una de les indústries que ha augmentat més espectacularment el seu desenvolupament ha estat la mèdico-farmaceutica gràcies als descobriments científics i a l'aplicació de nous tractaments.
Resumo:
Les persones amb un coeficient normal perceben de la mateixa manera els objectes grans que els petits, segons un estudi publicat a 'Current Biology'
Resumo:
L’atenció a la salut mental al llarg de la història es troba plena de desoris i de grans injustícies. Durant molts segles les persones amb malaltia mental han estat arraconades i menyspreades per la societat, que les ha reclòs en institucions psiquiàtriques tancades: els manicomis
Resumo:
En el present article es descriuen en primer lloc les tres tipologies de models de formació d'adults que han estat predominants en aquestes dues darreres dècades. A la segona part de l'article es comentaran alguns deis problemes que es donen i, es descriuran algunes propostes que a la llum de les diferents experiències poden ser més innovadores i poden resultar més convincents.
Resumo:
Learning objects have been the promise of providing people with high quality learning resources. Initiatives such as MIT Open-CourseWare, MERLOT and others have shown the real possibilities of creating and sharing knowledge through Internet. Thousands of educational resources are available through learning object repositories. We indeed live in an age of content abundance, and content can be considered as infrastructure for building adaptive and personalized learning paths, promoting both formal and informal learning. Nevertheless, although most educational institutions are adopting a more open approach, publishing huge amounts of educational resources, the reality is that these resources are barely used in other educational contexts. This paradox can be partly explained by the dificulties in adapting such resources with respect to language, e-learning standards and specifications and, finally, granularity. Furthermore, if we want our learners to use and take advantage of learning object repositories, we need to provide them with additional services than just browsing and searching for resources. Social networks can be a first step towards creating an open social community of learning around a topic or a subject. In this paper we discuss and analyze the process of using a learning object repository and building a social network on the top of it, with respect to the information architecture needed to capture and store the interaction between learners and resources in form of learning object metadata.