998 resultados para Historia Edad Antigua


Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

El propósito del presente trabajo es considerar las relaciones entre desarrollo, entendido como cambio evolutivo, y aprendizaje o cambio educativo, a la luz de las teorías de Piaget, Vygotski y el impacto que estas han tenido en los recientes avances alrededor de la Teoría de la Mente (TM). Entendemos por TM la capacidad que permite a las personas explicar su conducta y la de los demás basándose en la atribución de estados mentales, como creencias, deseos o intenciones. Se apunta que aprendizaje y desarrollo mantienen relaciones recíprocas que varían según el momento de desarrollo considerado. Así como las formas primigenias de cognición social como la atención conjunta apoyan la emergencia de las habilidades lingüísticas y el intercambio educativo, éste, a su vez, posibilita el desarrollode modos más complejos de cognición social, como la comprensión de la falsa creencia.Finalmente, se sugieren algunas actividades educativas encaminadas a fomentar laadquisición de la TM en la edad preescolar

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Suomen Etnomusikologisen Seuran 30-vuotisjuhlaseminaarissa pidetty esitelmä.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Esta comunicación presenta un modelo de experiencia didáctica que se plantea el uso de tecnologiasdigitales y de internet en clase de Geografía e Historia a partir de los recursos tecnològicoshabituales que existan en un centro educativo y de los enseres tecnológicos que utilizaregularmente el alumnado.Se basa en diversas ejemplificaciones realizadas entre 2006 y 2013 con alumnado deGeografia e Historia del Màster de Secundaria de la UB, del antiguo CAP de Geografia e Historiadel ICE de la UB y de los cursos para profesorado novel interino del Departament d’Ensenyamentde la Generalitat de Catalunya. En la diversas actuaciones se fueron considerando losdiversos parámetros que finalmente han servido para determinar la estructura de la experienciaaplicada recientemente. Se trata de un modelo basado en tres apartados, donde el primeroes una exposición de tipo transmisivo que plantearía la teoria de toda la actividad a realizar.Una segunda parte propone un trabajo en grupo de descubrimiento tecnológico y deplanificación temática de la materia. Finalmente se fija una exposición del producto realizado como resultado del aprendizaje conseguido.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Esta comunicación presenta un modelo de experiencia didáctica que se plantea el uso de tecnologiasdigitales y de internet en clase de Geografía e Historia a partir de los recursos tecnològicoshabituales que existan en un centro educativo y de los enseres tecnológicos que utilizaregularmente el alumnado.Se basa en diversas ejemplificaciones realizadas entre 2006 y 2013 con alumnado deGeografia e Historia del Màster de Secundaria de la UB, del antiguo CAP de Geografia e Historiadel ICE de la UB y de los cursos para profesorado novel interino del Departament d’Ensenyamentde la Generalitat de Catalunya. En la diversas actuaciones se fueron considerando losdiversos parámetros que finalmente han servido para determinar la estructura de la experienciaaplicada recientemente. Se trata de un modelo basado en tres apartados, donde el primeroes una exposición de tipo transmisivo que plantearía la teoria de toda la actividad a realizar.Una segunda parte propone un trabajo en grupo de descubrimiento tecnológico y deplanificación temática de la materia. Finalmente se fija una exposición del producto realizado como resultado del aprendizaje conseguido.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

La revisión de la bibliografía sobre la metodología del caso nos revelaque es frecuente la imprecisión en el uso de términos similares como:análisis de casos, estudios de caso, método de casos, etc. Estaconstatación ha de permitir, en adelante, evitar la ambigüedad y confusiónpermitiendo un uso preciso de los conceptos.La metodología del caso puede ser utilizada con distintas finalidades:contextualizar, explorar, ilustrar, demostrar, hacer comprobaciones ytomar decisiones, tanto con fines didácticos como de investigación...

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Exsiten muy pocos estudios que exploren la relación entre estilos de afrontamiento y la predisposición a los trastornos de alimentación (TCA) en muestras no clínicas yprincipalmente con personas adolescentes con personas adolescentes. Asimismo, no hemos localizado ningún estudio realizado con adolescentes que explore tal relaciónen función de la edad. Esta falta de estudios resulta especialmente relevante puesto que la adolescencia es unode los periodos de riesgo más importante y constituye el sector de la población a quien deben ir dirigidos los programas de prevención.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Des de la creació de l’Arxiu (1820) ha evolucionat el tractament dels seus fons. Segons els Reglaments (1822, 1879) els oficials descrivien els expedients, el registrador resseguia el tràmit, i l’arxiver controlava amb un registre la ubicació dels lligalls. Esteve (1836) inventarià la documentació antiga, estructurada en seccions per Quintana. Solà (1854), seguit per Rancés, classificà per negociats i matèries i millorà la descripció dels lligalls. Vallès confeccionà un Registre-inventari que descriu cronològicament per negociats i matèries tots els fons. També inventarià col•leccions i fons afegits, que la Mancomunitat destinà a l’IEC, la Biblioteca de Catalunya i els Museus d’Art. Paraules clau: Història d’arxius, arxivística catalana, arxivers catalans, Marià Vallès, Vallès i Vallès, arxiu, Diputació, Mancomunitat, Jacint Verdaguer, Prat de la Riba