912 resultados para Preferential-looking
Resumo:
As seen looking towards water from deck area.
Resumo:
As seen from jetty, looking back towards house and crow's nest.
Resumo:
Original living room, looking through to new outdoor room.
Resumo:
Looking towards section of original house from outdoor room area. Hand-made spotted gum columns on edge of outdoor room on right.
Resumo:
As seen from adjacent shed spaces, looking towards house and water beyond.
Resumo:
As seen from West, looking towards front of main pavilion. Standard roller doors to clerestory.
Resumo:
As seen from back of bedroom pavilion, looking towards main pavilion. Day bed alcove to bedroom in foreground.
Resumo:
As seen from East, looking along pavilions. Bedroom pavilion and day bed alcove in foreground.
Resumo:
As seen from living room interior, looking through to deck and kitchen beyond.
Resumo:
In an open channel, a hydraulic jump is the rapid transition from super- to sub-critical flow associated with strong turbulence and air bubble entrainment in the mixing layer. New experiments were performed at relatively large Reynolds numbers using phase-detection probes. Some new signal analysis provided characteristic air-water time and length scales of the vortical structures advecting the air bubbles in the developing shear flow. An analysis of the longitudinal air-water flow structure suggested little bubble clustering in the mixing layer, although an interparticle arrival time analysis showed some preferential bubble clustering for small bubbles with chord times below 3 ms. Correlation analyses yielded longitudinal air-water time scales Txx*V1/d1 of about 0.8 in average. The transverse integral length scale Z/d1 of the eddies advecting entrained bubbles was typically between 0.25 and 0.4, irrespective of the inflow conditions within the range of the investigations. Overall the findings highlighted the complicated nature of the air-water flow
Resumo:
Looking through to sitting room beyond.
Resumo:
As seen from the South-West, looking towards house. Carport structure and rainwater tank beyond.
Resumo:
Looking downhill to house during construction.
Resumo:
Looking uphill to house.
Resumo:
Some motor tasks can be completed, quite literally, with our eyes shut. Most people can touch their nose without looking or reach for an object after only a brief glance at its location. This distinction leads to one of the defining questions of movement control: is information gleaned prior to starting the movement sufficient to complete the task (open loop), or is feedback about the progress of the movement required (closed loop)? One task that has commanded considerable interest in the literature over the years is that of steering a vehicle, in particular lane-correction and lane-changing tasks. Recent work has suggested that this type of task can proceed in a fundamentally open loop manner [1 and 2], with feedback mainly serving to correct minor, accumulating errors. This paper reevaluates the conclusions of these studies by conducting a new set of experiments in a driving simulator. We demonstrate that, in fact, drivers rely on regular visual feedback, even during the well-practiced steering task of lane changing. Without feedback, drivers fail to initiate the return phase of the maneuver, resulting in systematic errors in final heading. The results provide new insight into the control of vehicle heading, suggesting that drivers employ a simple policy of “turn and see,” with only limited understanding of the relationship between steering angle and vehicle heading.