1000 resultados para Atraso escolar


Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

En el present treball s’aprofundeix en l’ús educatiu que es fa de l’hort a partir d’una recerca bibliogràfica sobre el tema i, també, a partir d’una recollida dades feta a dues escoles situades en contextos diferents: una pertany a un ambient rural amb només 21 alumnes en tota l’escola i l’altra a un ambient més urbà amb uns 200 infants només a parvulari. Per tal de dur a terme la teoria del treball i poder-la relacionar amb la pràctica, s’han utilitzat quatre àmbits que es creuen importants per tal que l’hort de l’escola funcioni correctament: gestió de la participació, l’hort: espais i cultiu, treball educatiu i paper del mestre. A partir d’aquesta relació s’han extret uns resultats i unes conclusions, de les quals les més rellevants són les següents: la gestió de l’hort no l’ha de portar a terme una sola persona sinó vàries, que aquest espai ha de servir per poder treballar la majoria d’àrees del currículum i que cada escola hauria d’adaptar l’hort segons les seves necessitats i interessos.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

L'adolescència és un període de transició entre la infància i l'edat adulta, una etapa del cicle de creixement que marca el final de la infància i anuncia la maduresa, aquest terme s'utilitza generalment per referir-se a una persona que es troba entre els 13 i 19 anys d'edat. En aquesta etapa s'inicia la maduració física, cognitiva, social i emocional del nen o nena en la cerca del camí cap a l'adultesa. Per a molts joves l'adolescència és un període d'incertesa i fins i tot de desesperació; per a uns altres, és una etapa d'amistats internes, de desvinculació de les lligadures amb els pares, de somnis sobre el futur, etc. El desenvolupament físic és només una part d'aquest procés de maduració, ja que els adolescents han de superar també una sèrie de canvis psicosocials, com per exemple: independència dels pares, aprenentatge d'estratègies de comunicació per relacionar-se de forma més madura, desenvolupament de les capacitats intel·lectuals, consolidació de la pròpia identitat, inici de les relacions sexuals, actituds i valors, control emocional, entre uns altres.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Postprint (published version)

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Es presenta un cas d’intervenció en educació per la sostenibilitat en un centre de preescolar d’Estelí ( Nicaragua) que es duu a terme creant una Agenda 21 Escolar. El marc teòric de la investigació es recolza en l’educació per la sostenibilitat i el marc metodològic es recolza en el model d’ Agenda 21 Escolar de Barcelona, adaptant-ho a la realitat del país i del centre de preescolar concret. Els resultats destaquen: una millora de la qualitat ambiental , una promoció de nous hàbits més sensibilitzats cap al medi ambient, una millora de la participació de la comunitat educativa i una bona cohesió de l’equip docent a partir del procés de creació de l’A21 E. Seguint com a model l’ A21 E de BCN i un cop fetes les adaptacions corresponents i creades les activitats dinamitzadores l’aplicació de la A21 E en preescolars d’ Estelí és una intervenció en educació per la sostenibilitat satisfactòria .

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Els infants adoptats presenten algunes especificitats que poden afectar la seva integració i evolució escolar (Palacios, 2007b; Dalen, 2007). Aquest ha estat el motiu de la present recerca que té tres finalitats: obtenir una descripció de les característiques dels infants adoptats en les etapes d’Educació Infantil i de Primària, mitjançant l’observació externa realitzada pels seus tutors d’aula; analitzar les necessitats dels mestres per atendre convenientment els nens procedents de l’adopció; posar els fonaments d’un programa escola-família per millorar l’adaptació escolar del nen i de la nena adoptats. En l’estudi van participar-hi 42 professors de 18 escoles de Catalunya. Les variables escollides per a analitzar els resultats del qüestionari, dissenyat per a la recerca, van ser les següents: etapa d’escolarització dels infants adoptats, sexe, anys d’escolarització dels menors, país d’origen dels nens, temps que fa que van ser adoptats i anys d’experiència del mestre. Atenent a aquestes variables, es va realitzar una anàlisi quantitativa i una anàlisi qualitativa. La quantitativa va consistir en un estudi descriptiu i un inferencial sobre la visió dels mestres dels infants adoptats i la relació amb la família adoptiva i la qualitativa en la categorització de les respostes dels mestres arran de les seves necessitats de formació per a atendre els menors procedents de l’adopció i del tractament a l’aula del fet adoptiu.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

La memoria es un proceso psicológico de gran importancia en la adquisición de mensajes publicitarios. Estudios previos han mostrado el valor de la memoria explícita o estratégica y de la memoria implícita. Otra variable que puede afectar al recuerdo es la disponibilidad de tiempo para el repaso de los estímulos presentados. Con el objetivo de verificar el efecto de la codificación y recuperación extrínsecas, así como del tiempo de exposición, se realizó un experimento con 84 estudiantes de Ciencias de la Información, que fueron asignados aleatoriamente a tres grupos. Se les presentó una serie de 30 anuncios publicitarios repartidos en tres listas que diferían en el tiempo de exposición. Los resultados muestran que los primeros y los últimos anuncios son los más recordados y que disminuye el recuerdo de los anuncios presentados en los primeros lugares, a medida que aumenta el tiempo de exposición de los mismos. Se interpretan los resultados en relación con la literatura previa y su posible aplicación al contexto escolar.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Este artículo presenta una reflexión sobre el currículo y el aprendizaje escolar en el marco de la nueva ecología del aprendizaje, propia de la sociedad de la información. Se presentan, en primer lugar, sus rasgos principales. Contrasta fuertemente con la de creación de los sistemas educativos actuales, que se sustenta en el modelo de escolarización universal. En la segunda parte, se platean los retos y desafíos de la educaciónescolar y los procesos de revisión curricular para afrontarlos y superarlos.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Teaching in sports with technique and tactic has brought different proposals in last years. From teaching games for understanding (TgfU), it has suggest some activities in teaching of sport, emphasizing the integrate teaching, and now is the moment for analysis and reflection, with enough time to prove if the contribution theory and practices help to know this model of teaching

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

La participación de los padres en los centros escolares suscita un interés reiteradamente manifestado desde diferentes instancias, principalmente la administración y las asociaciones de padres. Esta participación no es idéntica en toda la población; un grupo que en general ha sido etiquetado como poco participativo son los gitanos que, además, en muchos casos, presentan una valoración diferente de la utilidad de la escuela. Por otro lado, otro de los colectivos, muy hetereogéneo internamente como el de los gitanos, que despierta curiosidad en cuanto a la actitud respecto a la educación es el de los inmigrantes -principalmente los procedentes del llamado Tercer Mundo. Ante esto, a partir de estudios que tratan estos aspectos, algunos realizados por nosotros mismos, analizamos la perticipación de estas minorías en la escuela. Dados los resultados, fijándonos en la experiencia del Quebec, consideramos interesante desarrollar la figura del mediador como un profesional que puede incrementar la comunicación entre centro y familia, relación que se ve influenciada por barreras lingüísticas, socioeconómicas, culturales e institucionales.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

O objetivo deste trabalho foi avaliar o efeito de diferentes períodos de exposição da banana 'Maçã' a 50 ppb de 1-MCP (0; 3; 6; 9; 12 e 24 horas) sobre sua vida pós-colheita e qualidade. Após exposição ao 1-MCP, os frutos verde-maturos foram armazenados por 30 dias em câmaras com temperatura de 13ºC ± 0,5 e umidade relativa de 95%. Em seguida, as bananas foram armazenadas à temperatura de 20ºC ± 1, até amarelecimento completo da casca. A exposição de banana 'Maçã' a 50 ppb de 1-MCP, por 9 horas, retardou em 7 dias o seu amadurecimento, em comparação a frutos não expostos ao 1-MCP, após 30 dias de armazenamento refrigerado (13ºC), sem prejuízos à sua aparência e composição química. A exposição de banana 'Maçã' a 50 ppb de 1-MCP, por 3 e 6 horas, não estendeu sua vida pós-colheita, tampouco alterou sua composição química. Embora a exposição de banana 'Maçã' a 50 ppb de 1-MCP, por 12 e 24 horas, tenha retardado o seu amadurecimento, promoveu alterações indesejáveis na casca do fruto. Logo, a aplicação de 50 ppb de 1-MCP, por 9 horas, antes da refrigeração, constitui-se numa alternativa viável para prolongar o período de comercialização da banana.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

El incremento de la diversidad cultural ha comportado que se elaboren diferentes actuaciones para aproximar posiciones y mejorar la comunicación entre progenitores y profesionales de la escuela, así como para facilitar la adaptación del currículum a la diversidad cultural. Una de las opciones tomadas, con cierto éxito pero también con errores, es el llamado mediador intercultural. Sin pretender que ésta sea la solución idónea en todas las situaciones y que represente la presencia de un nuevo profesional en los centros educativos, la experiencia de Montreal -y, como veremos, la de alguna otra ciudad de Canadá- y del conjunto de Quebec es ilustrativa de lo que puede representar esta figura. Y más cuando en algunas Comunidades Autónomas, por ejemplo Cataluña, aparecen espontáneamente o potenciadas por diferentes instituciones, estos "profesionales".

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

El diseño de los planes de estudio para las nuevas titulaciones de grado de Magisterio constituye, por su carácter profesionalizador basado en competencias y la ampliación de los estudios a cuatro cursos, una oportunidad para las facultades de educación de mejora de la calidad de la formación inicial de maestros. Gran parte del incremento de los créditos se destina a la ampliación de los periodos de prácticas en los centros escolares colaboradores. Se trata de un escenario de cambio profundo que deberá replantear la organización y revisar los modelos. La experiencia en Quebec nos proporciona elementos de análisis y de reflexión con la finalidad de llegar a orientaciones y a conclusiones transferibles a la organización de estos planes de estudio y para integrar el prácticum en una visión global e interdisciplinaria de la titulación.