120 resultados para Personal Autonomy


Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

La preservació digital (PD) s'ha convertit en un problema persistent per a tots els que vulguin conservar la seva informació digital, garantir el seu estat i consultar aquest informació en el transcurs del temps. Fins ara només grans institucions amb coneixement expert i eines especialitzades han pogut fer front a aquest problema, però la preservació digital no pot ser abordada per una sola institució o nació. Les biblioteques, arxius i altres institucions de conservació de la memòria comparteixen aquest repte de la mateixa manera que els col•leccionistes i creadors, que ho fan a títol individual.L’objectiu del projecte és crear l'aplicació Pyramid que està concebuda com una eina de suport orientada a l'usuari domèstic (sense coneixements tècnics ni de preservació) per a la preservació a mig i llarg termini de col•leccions digitals, texts i vídeos, tal que funcioni com un antivirus (en BackGround) i preservi la informació sense requerir un cost addicional a l'ordinador i que l'usuari no noti cap molèstia a l'hora de fer les seves tasques diàries

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

What determines risk-bearing capacity and the amount of leverage in financial markets? Thispaper uses unique micro-data on collateralized lending contracts during a period of financialdistress to address this question. An investor syndicate speculating in English stocks wentbankrupt in 1772. Using hand-collected information from Dutch notarial archives, we examinechanges in lenders' behavior following exposure to potential (but not actual) losses. Before thedistress episode, financiers that lent to the ill-fated syndicate were indistinguishable from therest. Afterwards, they behaved differently: they lent with much higher haircuts. Only lendersexposed to the failed syndicate altered their behavior. The differential change is remarkable sincethe distress was public knowledge, and because none of the lenders suffered actual losses ? allfinanciers were repaid in full. Interest rates were also unaffected; the market balanced solelythrough changes in collateral requirements. Our findings are consistent with a heterogeneousbeliefs-interpretation of leverage. They also suggest that individual experience can modify thelevel of leverage in a market quickly.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

El objetivo principal de este proyecto es desarrollar más a fondo y consolidar los objetivos formativos y competencias del grado en Estudios Ingleses a través de un trabajo de escritura creativa, en concreto, la creación de una obra teatral original. Este trabajo consiste en, por un lado, una introducción al Naturalismo, como género literario en un marco cultural y metodológico, y por otro lado, la creación personal de una obra naturalista que incluye la primera parte del manuscrito. Este proyecto incluye un proceso completo de creatividad justificado gracias a las principales teorías de algunos escritores naturalistas, como: Émile Zola – como precursor del Naturalismo – y August Strindberg como uno de los mayores mentores de este género literario. Los resultados obtenidos en este trabajo son: la creación de una obra teatral original, una especialización en el movimiento Naturalista aplicado al teatro contemporáneo, y un profundo conocimiento de la lengua inglesa a través de la perspectiva de un dramaturgo.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Etimológicamente, esclerosis significa endurecimiento (skerós es "endurecmiento patológico" y osis, "enfermedad") y hace referencia al estado de la médula espinal en las fases avanzadas de la enfermedad. Lateral significa "al lado" y pone de manifiesto la ubicación del daño en la médula espinal. Por último, el término amiotrófica significa "sin nutrición muscular" y se refiere a la pérdida de señales que los nervios envían normalmente a los músculos. La Esclerosis Lateral Amiotrófica (ELA) es la enfermedad más grave de un grupo de dolencias que tienen síntomas similares y son conocidas como "enfermedades de motoneuronas": la Atrofia Muscular Juvenil o Enfermedad de Kugelberg Welander, la Atrofia Muscular Infantil o Enfermedad de Werdnig Hoffmann, la parálisis bulbar progresiva, la amiotrofia focal benigna, la esclerosis lateral primaria y la atrofia muscular espinobulbar o Enfermedad de Kennedy.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Se presenta un modelo de análisis del comportamiento informacional global de un colectivo de individuos (estudiantes de la Universitat Oberta de Catalunya) que tienen una percepción positiva sobre el uso de las tecnologías de la información y la comunicación y que realizan un uso intensivo de las mismas.A partir de una aproximación cualitativa, mediante 24 entrevistas y un posterior análisis del contenido, se identifican cuatro perfiles distintos de gestión de la información personal (reactivo, pasivo, exhaustivo y proactivo) en base a diez variables subyacentes (acceso, gestión y usos de la información, competenciasinformacionales, perfil cognitivo, actitud, percepción de las TIC, ámbito académico, profesional y de la vida diaria) y se ponen derelieve las diferencias de comportamiento informacional dependiendo del ámbito en el que se encuentren. La identificación de los perfiles es un estadio básico del diseño centrado en los usuarios que facilita la realización de intervenciones específicas para cada tipo de usuario, respetando requerimientos de herramientasy procesos para que puedan desarrollar su comportamiento informacional de forma eficiente y eficaz.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Institutional digital repositories are a basic piece to provide preservation and reutilization of learning resources. However, their creation and maintenance is usually performed following a top-down approach, causing limitations in the search and reutilization of learning resources. In order to avoid this problem we propose to use web 2.0 functionalities. In this paper we present how tagging can be used to enhance the search and reusability functionalities of institutional learning repositories as well as promoting their usage. The paper also describes the evaluation process that was performed in a pilot experience involving open educational resources.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Definir una estratègia per a la nostra marca personal com a forma d’aproximació dels nostres valors, atributs i objectius cap al nostre públic objectiu és el plantejament d’aquest projecte. Amb aquesta finalitat es desenvoluparà una estratègia de comunicació digital, per l’actor català Albert Carbó, que comprengui les seves necessitats per arribar a complir amb els seus objectius.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Els serve is de protecció a les persones en situació de dependencia configuraran I'any 2007, segons s'ha anunciat, un nou sistema d'atenció que comporta esperances pero també reptes tant a escala prestacional com de qualitat deIs serveis. Aquest article exposa els trets generals del projecte de llei estatal de "Promoció de I'autonomia personal i atenció a les persones en situació de dependencia", el contingut de I'actual sistema de serveis socials en re lació amb la tematica i les exigencies d'una veritable promoció i atenció de qualitat.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

L"objectiu de la contribució que presentem és força senzill d"entendre: hem volgut donar forma a una activitat viscuda per recordar-la i per aprofitar-la quan l"haguem de menester. Com ho hem fet?, doncs materialitzant uns fets, uns pensaments i unes idees en forma de document, el qual a hores d"ara ja forma part del nostre fons personal. La finalitat del document, però, va més enllà: volem donar a conèixer la vàlua dels fons personals com a recurs estratègic per a l"aprenentatge de l"Arxivística. Hem volgut compartir la nostra experiència en aquest canal de comunicació professional perquè pensem, modestament, que treballs com aquest poden ser noves formes d"avançar en el progrés teòric de l"Arxivística.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

La realidad socio-educativa ha pasado de una situación estable, uniforme y continuista a otra en la cual se destaca la rapidez, la diversidad y la necesidad de cambios. Basados en este nuevo contexto, presentamos una actuación estratégica cooperativa para desarrollar nuevas competencias de formación para nuestros futuros docentes. Se han establecido diferentes apartados: 1) Un análisis comparativo entre el contexto educativo actual y el papel estratégico, tanto de formación como metodológico, que han de adoptar los docentes. 2) Una reflexión sobre las nuevas competencias de desarrollo personal y social que a través del área de educación física han de constituir los nuevos currículos de primaria y secundaria. 3) La presentación y aplicación de una propuesta cooperativa que modifica los diseños tradicionales de intervención en el proceso de aprendizaje de los alumnos y facilita el trabajo desde una perspectiva, más dialogada y participativa, basada en la resolución de problemas. Una propuesta que favorece los cambios y muestra una intervención estratégica que facilita la consecución de nuevas competencias de desarrollo personal y social desde la educación física.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

El artículo plantea un análisis empírico sobre las posibilidades de aplicación de las nuevas tecnologías de la información al proceso de reclutamiento de personal. Las competencias sociales y cognitivas que requieren las nuevas formas de organización de la producción plantean nuevos métodos de aprendizaje y la actualización del desarrollo de capacidades y comportamientos. Se trata de renovar y completar las competencias profesionales en un proceso permanente, que implica la adopción de una política de reclutamiento orientada por la consideración del conocimiento como elemento diferenciador de competitividad empresarial y de creación de riqueza.

Relevância:

20.00% 20.00%

Publicador:

Resumo:

Este trabajo aborda la formación de los grupos dirigentes absolutista y liberal en la Lleida del último cuarto de siglo XVIII y primer tercio de siglo XIX. Siguiendo los principales hechos políticos de este periodo, dibujamos el enfrentamiento progresivo de ambos grupos en la comunidad. Asimismo, reflejamos una distinta adscripción socioprofesional según fuese su filiación política. De esta manera, constatamos que las filas absolutistas se nutrieron de las familias con más influencia social (pequeña nobleza local, ciudadanos honrados, notables y doctores) y los campesinos; mientras que los liberales concentraron las simpatías de los artesanos, hacendados sin privilegio, profesionales liberales e intelectuales.