313 resultados para Joves -- Esbarjo
Resumo:
The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.
Resumo:
The purpose of this article is to treat a currently much debated issue, the effects of age on second language learning. To do so, we contrast data collected by our research team from over one thousand seven hundred young and adult learners with four popular beliefs or generalizations, which, while deeply rooted in this society, are not always corroborated by our data.Two of these generalizations about Second Language Acquisition (languages spoken in the social context) seem to be widely accepted: a) older children, adolescents and adults are quicker and more efficient at the first stages of learning than are younger learners; b) in a natural context children with an early start are more liable to attain higher levels of proficiency. However, in the context of Foreign Language Acquisition, the context in which we collect the data, this second generalization is difficult to verify due to the low number of instructional hours (a maximum of some 800 hours) and the lower levels of language exposure time provided. The design of our research project has allowed us to study differences observed with respect to the age of onset (ranging from 2 to 18+), but in this article we focus on students who began English instruction at the age of 8 (LOGSE Educational System) and those who began at the age of 11 (EGB). We have collected data from both groups after a period of 200 (Time 1) and 416 instructional hours (Time 2), and we are currently collecting data after a period of 726 instructional hours (Time 3). We have designed and administered a variety of tests: tests on English production and reception, both oral and written, and within both academic and communicative oriented approaches, on the learners' L1 (Spanish and Catalan), as well as a questionnaire eliciting personal and sociolinguistic information. The questions we address and the relevant empirical evidence are as follows: 1. "For young children, learning languages is a game. They enjoy it more than adults."Our data demonstrate that the situation is not quite so. Firstly, both at the levels of Primary and Secondary education (ranging from 70.5% in 11-year-olds to 89% in 14-year-olds) students have a positive attitude towards learning English. Secondly, there is a difference between the two groups with respect to the factors they cite as responsible for their motivation to learn English: the younger students cite intrinsic factors, such as the games they play, the methodology used and the teacher, whereas the older students cite extrinsic factors, such as the role of their knowledge of English in the achievement of their future professional goals. 2 ."Young children have more resources to learn languages." Here our data suggest just the opposite. The ability to employ learning strategies (actions or steps used) increases with age. Older learners' strategies are more varied and cognitively more complex. In contrast, younger learners depend more on their interlocutor and external resources and therefore have a lower level of autonomy in their learning. 3. "Young children don't talk much but understand a lot"This third generalization does seem to be confirmed, at least to a certain extent, by our data in relation to the analysis of differences due to the age factor and productive use of the target language. As seen above, the comparably slower progress of the younger learners is confirmed. Our analysis of interpersonal receptive abilities demonstrates as well the advantage of the older learners. Nevertheless, with respect to passive receptive activities (for example, simple recognition of words or sentences) no great differences are observed. Statistical analyses suggest that in this test, in contrast to the others analyzed, the dominance of the subjects' L1s (reflecting a cognitive capacity that grows with age) has no significant influence on the learning process. 4. "The sooner they begin, the better their results will be in written language"This is not either completely confirmed in our research. First of all, we perceive that certain compensatory strategies disappear only with age, but not with the number of instructional hours. Secondly, given an identical number of instructional hours, the older subjects obtain better results. With respect to our analysis of data from subjects of the same age (12 years old) but with a different number of instructional hours (200 and 416 respectively, as they began at the ages of 11 and 8), we observe that those who began earlier excel only in the area of lexical fluency. In conclusion, the superior rate of older learners appears to be due to their higher level of cognitive development, a factor which allows them to benefit more from formal or explicit instruction in the school context. Younger learners, however, do not benefit from the quantity and quality of linguistic exposure typical of a natural acquisition context in which they would be allowed to make use of implicit learning abilities. It seems clear, then, that the initiative in this country to begin foreign language instruction earlier will have positive effects only if it occurs in combination with either higher levels of exposure time to the foreign language, or, alternatively, with its use as the language of instruction in other areas of the curriculum.
Resumo:
El proyecto consiste en el desarrollo de una aplicación para teléfonos móviles Android, dirigida hacia personas mayores o más jóvenes con alguna discapacidad, siempre que no sea muy severa, que tienen sus capacidades de orientación mermadas, lo que provoca que cuando salen de casa puedan perderse y no sepan cómo regresar.
Resumo:
Fa dies, l'Institut d'Estadística de Catalunya va fer públic el resultat de l'Enquesta a la joventut de Catalunya 2012. Entre totes les dades que hi consten, n'hi ha una que em va cridar l'atenció, possiblement perquè acabava de llegir un parell d'articles científics sobre el tema: un 4,1% dels joves entre 15 i 19 anys consideren que la seva feina és molt estressant, i un 36,6% que ho és força.
Resumo:
L’ús que fan els estudiants de segon cicle d’ESO de la ciutat de Barcelona de les diferents eines de Facebook, per a crear i consumir continguts generats per peers. Quines motivacions hi ha rere l’ús de cada eina i les relacions entre l’ús d’unes determinades eines per part dels usuaris i la concepció que tenen d’ells mateixos pel què fa a la seva popularitat.
Prestacions en benefici de la comunitat i realització de tasques socioeducatives en justícia juvenil
Resumo:
Els joves als quals se’ls ha imposat una mesura de realització de tasques socioeducatives (RTS) o de prestacions en benefici de la comunitat (PBC) presenten un alt fracàs escolar, majoritàriament amb baix rendiment i abandonament dels estudis en la meitat dels casos. Un de cada tres joves prové d’una família amb algun tipus de problemàtica específica o una economia insuficient. Els resultats indiquen, però, que els joves que realitzen tasques socioeducatives solen acumular més problemàtiques que els de PBC, ja sigui a nivell personal, familiar o social. Quant al perfil penal i criminològic, el 63% dels nois i noies d’aquestes dues mesures té antecedents. La taxa de reincidència de RTS és del 31,1% i la de PBC del 25,2%. Alguns dels factors que els joves reincidents mostren en més proporció són tenir un grup d’iguals dissocial, estar en contacte amb els serveis socials o tenir antecedents. Els reincidents de RTS també presenten més proporció de consum de tòxics, problemes de salut mental, fracàs escolar i una ocupació del temps desestructurada i amb conductes de risc. Comparant aquests resultats amb estudis respecte altres programes i mesures de Justícia juvenil podem concloure que la taxa de reincidència global a Catalunya és del 28,9% i l’específica de Medi obert és del 27,8%.
Resumo:
En el procés d’envelliment es produeixen canvis neuropsicològics, de forma que algunes funcions cognitives es deterioren progressivament al llarg de tota la vida adulta, altres es mantenen estables fins edats avançades, mentre que d'altres poden inclús millorar. Està ben establert que l'envelliment afecta la memòria, atenció, funcions visoperceptives, velocitat de processament, funcions executives, així com algunes funcions lingüístiques. Aquests canvis en les funcions cognitives tenen el seu correlat en el deteriorament cerebral que es produeix en l’envelliment. Estudis recents amb tècniques de neuroimatge han mostrat que mentre que els adults joves presenten un patró fortament lateralitzat en l'hemisferi esquerre en tasques de llenguatge i de memòria verbal, les persones grans tendeixen a mostrar patrons bilaterals. Pel que fa al processament del llenguatge, es postula la presència de pèrdua de recursos, alentiment en el processament de la informació, falta d’inhibició, dèficits en la transmissió, deteriorament de la memòria de treball o dèficits sensorials i perceptius. Algunes funcions lingüístiques s’afecten més que d’altres, però hi ha evidència de la presència d’un deteriorament del llenguatge associat a l’edat, amb afectació del lèxic, sintaxi i organització discursiva. Es fonamental tenir en compte aquests canvis, per tal d’afavorir un envelliment satisfactori i millorar la qualitat de vida, tant de la persona gran com dels seus familiars.
Resumo:
Per què la ciència no arriba a la societat? Les matriculacions en ciències disminueixen, el periodisme científic és una rara avis, els ajuts públics a la divulgació científica cauen en picat. És possible fer atractiva la ciència als joves com a referent professional de futur o fins i tot, com passa ara amb l"esport, protagonista del lleure familiar?
Resumo:
Award-winning
Resumo:
La forma en que adolescentes y jóvenes utilizan las tecnologías de la información y la comunicación es diferente a la de la población adulta. Las prácticas son más intensivas y extensivas entre l@s más jóvenes, tal y como muestran las estadísticas disponibles. Este resultado, ampliamente conocido, nos lleva a preguntarnos ¿qué implicaciones tienen estas prácticas comunicativas de las generaciones más jóvenes?Tomando como punto de partida algunos estudios que analizan la situación, tanto en España como en otros países, revisaremos los cambios que ya se están produciendo en ámbitos que van desde el aprendizaje a la socialización. Revisaremos, asimismo, algunas propuestas que intentan dar una explicación teórica global de las prácticas y las actitudes comunicativas de jóvenes y adolescentes. En el análisis tendremos en cuenta el creciente proceso de individualización de nuestras sociedades y la presencia habitual de comunidades de práctica permanentemente conectadas que caracterizan a estas generaciones.
Resumo:
La torre s’aixeca en el front del carrer Llacuna cantonada Gran Via. Es tracta d’un volum prismàtic unitari i compacte de plantes rectangulars.La seva construcció de vidre, planxa i formigó ofereixen la imatge exterior de l’Edifici amb una textura modular, repetitiva i austera que manifesta ña seva condició de lloc pel treball i residència de múltiples i joves empreses i usuaris en trànsit.
Resumo:
La formació dels joves universitaris del segle XXI, immersos en un món global i en la societat de la informació i del coneixement, no es pot concebre sense una clara referència a la dimensió internacional. Mai com fins ara s’havien tingut a l’abast tants recursos de connexió per apropar els pobles i la seva gent, facilitant la comunicació a diversos nivells: informatiu, social, professional i personal. Enguany s’acompleix el vintè aniversari de la nostra plena integració a Europa
Resumo:
L'educació, la salut i la protecció són els drets més coneguts i respectats pels adults, pero hi ha un ciar deficit en re lació amb els drets relac ionats amb I 'exercici de la participació i l' expressió. El coneixement deis drets d'ex pressió i participació de la in fancia es dóna més entre els polítics i educadors socialsqueentreels pares i professors,el que seria un indicador d' una manca greu de polítiques insütucionals i culturals. Les polítiques de família reforcen a: la institució familiar i, per tant, poden incloure els in fants sense donar- los protagonisme. Per contra, les polítiques específicament adreçades a la infancia i adolescencia afavoreixen el procés d' autonomització i responsabilitzacióen el desenvolupament dels infants i joves.
Resumo:
Las inscripciones sobre el mundo a través de dibujos, notaciones, tablas, diagramas, etc. son una parte esencial de toda investigación científica (Latour y Woolgar, 1979). Latour define"inscripción" como medio a través del cual las propiedades del mundo pueden fijarse, lo que permitiría centrarse en aquellos aspectos esenciales para construir conocimiento. Por otro lado, la investigación científica tiene lugar mediante procesos que implican el planteamiento de preguntas e hipótesis, el diseño de experimentos, hacer predicciones, observaciones y mediciones, registrar datos y interpretar resultados, consultar y analizar el registro de los datos, verificar y reaccionar ante los datos discrepantes, elaborar argumentos, construir varias representaciones de los datos y coordinarlas con teorías previas (Duschl et al, 2007). Este trabajo tiene como objetivo el análisis de la regulación mutua entre las prácticas inscripcionales y la investigación científica en alumnos de final de enseñanza primaria. Esta relación la ilustramos mediante un análisis microgenético de estudio de caso.
Resumo:
My study is based on an ethnography of two groups of young people from working-class neighbourhoods in Barcelona. I was interested in researching the impact of Catalan language policies on the identities of young people of Spanish-speaking immigrant families. I sought to go beyond the constraints of traditional structuralist approaches in Sociolinguistics in order to make my analysis relevant to people working for gender equality, the promotion of the Catalan language, or other social causes. I combine ideas from Bakhtin, Bourdieu, Fairclough, Foucault and Goffman to build a dialectical, historical, process-centred perspective that conceptualises practices in terms of social and political struggles.I analyse young people's peer-group activities in terms of their significance for the construction of gender identities. I propose a variety of forms of masculinity and femininity according to the various ways in which members organised their gender displays in face-to-face interaction.I also show how their use of argot and dialectal Spanish was part of the processes whereby members defined their relationships, constructed particular subject positions in interaction and struggled to legitimate their own values.I explore the meanings constructed through Catalan and Spanish by looking into the code-switching practices of my participants. I analysed their talk in terms of narratives that present particular sequential dramatisations of events for conversational audiences. These narratives follow the expressive intention of the author, and are populated with multiple voices of animated characters. I argue that, in the groups I studied, Catalan was generally not used to animate the voices that were central to the identities of the peer-group, and particularly to masculine identities.In order to contextualise these practices within the wider society, I also look into the processes of language choice in face-to-face encounters. I argue that existing conventions made it difficult for people to find opportunities to speak Catalan. I also pointed to the difficulties that my participants had to find employment, which were particularly acute amongst the more politically aware individuals. I conclude that these young working-class people had little possibilities of investing in more egalitarian forms of identity given their lack of resources and opportunities to develop their identities in other social spaces, such as the workplace.