The use of cues for attention in ancient Greek art: aspects that influence concentration in the work of art and its elements


Autoria(s): Duarte, Antonio M.; Universidad de Lisboa; Stefanakis, Manolis I.; University of the Aegean
Data(s)

10/09/2015

Resumo

The goal of this study is to identify cues for the cognitive process of attention in ancient Greek art, aiming to find confirmation of its possible use by ancient Greek audiences and artists. Evidence of cues that trigger attention’s psychological dispositions was searched through content analysis of image reproductions of ancient Greek sculpture and fine vase painting from the archaic to the Hellenistic period - ca. 7th -1st cent. BC. Through this analysis, it was possible to observe the presence of cues that trigger orientation to the work of art (i.e. amplification, contrast, emotional salience, simplification, symmetry), of a cue that triggers a disseminate attention to the parts of the work (i.e. distribution of elements) and of cues that activate selective attention to specific elements in the work of art (i.e. contrast of elements, salient color, central positioning of elements, composition regarding the flow of elements and significant objects). Results support the universality of those dispositions, probably connected with basic competencies that are hard-wired in the nervous system and in the cognitive processes.

El objetivo de este estudio es identificar las señales para el proceso cognitivo de la atención en el arte Griego antiguo, con la intención de detectar su posible uso por los antiguos griegos (público y artistas). La evidencia de señales que desencadenan las disposiciones psicológicas de la atención se buscó mediante el análisis de contenido de reproducciones de imágenes de arte Griego antiguo (escultura y pintura de vasos desde el período arcaico hasta el período Helenístico – ca. 7º-1º siglo AC). Por medio de este análisis fue posible observar la presencia de señales que condicionan la orientación para la obra de arte (i.e. amplificación, contraste, énfasis emocional, simplificación, simetría), de una señal que provoca una atención diseminada a las partes de la obra (i.e. distribución de los elementos) y de señales que activan una atención selectiva a las partes de la obra (i.e. contraste de los elementos, color saliente, posicionamiento central de los elementos, composición con respecto al fluir de los elementos y objetos significativos). Los resultados apoyan la universalidad de esas disposiciones, probablemente relacionado con competencias básicas del sistema nervioso y de los procesos cognitivos.

Formato

application/pdf

Identificador

http://revistas.ucm.es/index.php/ARIS/article/view/46654

10.5209/rev_ARIS.2015.v27.n3.46654

Publicador

Ediciones Complutense

Relação

http://revistas.ucm.es/index.php/ARIS/article/view/46654/46413

/*ref*/Arnheim, R. (1974). Art and visual perception: a psychology of the creative eye. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.

/*ref*/Arnheim, R. (1986). New essays on the psychology of art: Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.

/*ref*/Arnheim, R. (1988). The power of the center: A study of composition in the visual arts. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.

/*ref*/Beazley, J.D. (1986). The development of the attic black-figure (Revised ed.). Berkeley: University of California Press

/*ref*/Behrens, R. (2002). False colors: art, design and modern camouflage. Iowa: Bobolink Books

/*ref*/Behrens, R. (2009). Camoupedia: A compendium of research on art, architecture and camouflage. Iowa: Bobolink Books

/*ref*/Berlyne, D. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts

/*ref*/Boardman, J. (1989). Athenian red-figure vases: The archaic period. London: Thames and Hudson

/*ref*/Boardman, J. (1991). Athenian black-figure vases. London: Thames and Hudson.

/*ref*/Boardman, J. (2000). Athenian red-figure vases: The archaic period. London: Thames and Hudson

/*ref*/Brinkmann, V., Primavesi, O. & Hollein, M. (2010). Circumlitio. The polychromy of antique and medieval sculptur. Munich: Hirmer.

/*ref*/Buranelli, F., Liverani, P. & Nesselrath, A. (Eds.) (2006). Laocoonte. Alle origini dei musei vaticani. Vatican: Museo Vaticano.

/*ref*/Chatterjee, A. (2013). The aesthetic brain – How we evolve to desire beauty and enjoy art. Oxford: Oxford University Press

/*ref*/Chua, H. F., Boland, J. E. & Nisbett R. E. (2005). Cultural variation in eye movements during scene perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 35, 12.629-12.633. Retrieved from http://www.pnas.org/content/102/35/12629.full.pdf

/*ref*/Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: embodiment, action and cognition extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

/*ref*/Clark, A. & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind, Analysis, 58, 7-19. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/analys/58.1.7

/*ref*/Cook, R. M. & Dupont, P. (2003). East Greek Pottery. London: Routledge.

/*ref*/CVA online: The Corpus Vasorum Antiquorum Project on line (http://www.beazley.ox.ac.uk/cva/proejectpages/cva1.htm

/*ref*/Dissanayake, E. (1992). Homo aestheticus: Where art comes from and why. New York: Free Press.

/*ref*/Damasio, A. (2010). Self comes to mind – Constructing the conscious brain. New York: Pantheon Books.

/*ref*/Donald, M. (2006). Art and cognitive evolution. In M. Turner (Ed.). The artful mind - Cognitive science and the riddle of human creativity (pp.3-20). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

/*ref*/Duffy, B. (2005). The analysis of documentary evidences. In J. Bell (Ed.) Doing your research project, (4th ed). (pp.122 -135). Berkshire: Open University Press.

/*ref*/Feagin, S. L. (1999). Aesthetics. In R. Audi (Ed.) The Cambridge dictionary of philosophy (2nd ed.) (pp.11-13). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

/*ref*/Freiwald, W. A., Tsao, D.Y. & Livingstone, M. S. (2009). A face feature space in the macaque temporal lobe. Nature Neuroscience, 12(9), 1187–1196. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2363

/*ref*/Gombrich, E. H. (1977). Art and illusion – A study in the psychology of pictorial representation. Oxford: Phaidon Press.

/*ref*/Gonçalves, A., Moura, J. P., Magalhães, L & Chalmers, A. (2013). Perceptual images of Conimbriga using High Dynamic Range. Journal of Archaeological Science, 40(1), 116-128. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jas.2012.06.024

/*ref*/Haskell, F. & Penny, N. (1981). Taste and the antique: The lure of classical sculpture, 1500-1900. New Haven: Yale University Press

/*ref*/Hatzivassiliou, E. (2010). Athenian black figure iconography between 510 and 475 B.C. (Tübinger Archäologische Forschungen 6). Rahden/Westf.: Verlag Marie Leidorf.

/*ref*/Hemingway, S. (2004). The horse and jockey from Artemision: A bronze equestrian monument of the hellenistic period. Berkeley: University of California Press.

/*ref*/Kandel, E. R. (2012). The age of insight: The quest to understand the unconscious in art, mind, and brain, from Vienna 1900 to the present. New York: Random House

/*ref*/Leder, H. (2013). Next Steps in Neuroaesthetics: Which processes and processing stages to study? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(1), 27–37. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031585

/*ref*/Leder, H., Belke, B., Oeberst, A. & Augustin, D. (2004). A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 489–508.doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/0007126042369811

/*ref*/Levinsson, J. (2011). Beauty is not one: The irreducible variety of visual beauty. In E. Schellekens & P. Goldie (Eds.). The aesthetic mind – Philosophy and psychology (pp. 90-207). Oxford: Oxford University Press

/*ref*/Levy, K. (2012). An artistic exploration of inattention blindness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 1-14. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00174

/*ref*/Lewis-Williams, D. J. (2002). The mind in the cave: Consciousness and the origins of art. London: Thames & Hudson.

/*ref*/Lewis-Williams, D. J. & Pearce, D. (2005). Inside the neolithic mind: Consciousness, cosmos and the realm of the gods. London: Thames & Hudson.

/*ref*/Locher,P. J. & Nodine, C. F. (1987). Symmetry catches the eye. In J. K. O’Regan & A. Lévy-Schoen (Eds.) Eye movements: From physiology to cognition (pp. 353-361). Holland: Elsevier Science Publications.

/*ref*/Malafouris, L. & Renfrew, C. (2010). The cognitive life of things: Archaeology, material engagement and the extended mind. In L. Malafouris & C. Renfrew (Eds.). The cognitive life of things. Recasting the boundaries of the mind (pp.1-12). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, University of Cambridge.

/*ref*/Martindale, C. (1990). The clockwork muse: The predictability of artistic change. New York: Basic Books.

/*ref*/Mather, G. (2014). The psychology of visual art – Eye, brain and art. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

/*ref*/Miles, M. B. & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis: an expanded sourcebook, 2nd ed. California: Sage Publications.

/*ref*/Mithen, S. J (1990) Thoughtful foragers: A study of prehistoric decision making. Cambridge; New York: Cambridge University Press.

/*ref*/Mithen, S. (1996). Prehistory of the mind: A search for the origins of art, religion and science. London and New York: Thames & Hudson.

/*ref*/Molnar, F. (1981). About the role of visual exploration in aesthetics. In H. I. Day (Ed.) Advances in intrinsic motivation and aesthetics (385-413). New York: Plenum Press

/*ref*/Moore, M.B. (1997). Attic red-figured and white-ground pottery. The Athenian agora XXX. Princeton: American School of Classical Studies at Athens.

/*ref*/Muzzupappa, M. A., Gallo, R. M., Mattanò, C., Ruggiero, & Bruno, F. (2012). a complete morphological study of the right hand of bronzo “A” di Riace. International Journal of Heritage in the Digital Era, 1(1), 55-60.

/*ref*/Nodine, C. F. & Kundel, H. L (1997). Al Hirschfeld’s NINA as a prototype search task for studying perceptual error in radiology. Proc. SPIE 3036, Medical Imaging 1997: Image Perception, 308. doi : http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/12.271306

/*ref*/Panzanelli, R., Schmidt, E. & Lapatin, K. (Eds.) (2008). The color of life: Polychromy in sculpture from antiquity to the present. Los Angeles: Getty Research Institute

/*ref*/Picard, C. (1932). Le guerrier blessé de l’Agora des Italiens a Délos, Bulletin de Correspondance Hellénique, 56, 491-530. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3406/bch.1932.2850

/*ref*/Pollitt, J.J. (1986). Art in the hellenistic age. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

/*ref*/Quiroga, R. Q. & Pedreira, C. (2011). How do we see art: An eye-tracker study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 1-9. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00098

/*ref*/Ramachandran V. S. (2003). The Emerging Mind. The BBC Reith Lectures. London: BBC and Profile Books.

/*ref*/Ramachandran V. S. (2011). The tell-tale brain – A neuroscientist’s quest for what makes us human. New York - London: W. W. Norton & Company.

/*ref*/Ramachandran, V. S. & Hirstein, W. (1999). The science of art. A neurological theory of aesthetic experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(6-7), 15–51.

/*ref*/Ramachandran, V. S. & Seckel, E. (2012). Neurology of visual aesthetics: Indian nymphs, modern art, and sexy beacks. In A. P. Shimamura & S. E. Palmer (Eds.). Aesthetic science – Connecting minds, brain and experience (pp. 375-390). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

/*ref*/Seymour, H. (1989). Laocoon rerestored. American Journal of Archaeology, 93(3), 417-422

/*ref*/Shimamura, A. (2013). Experiencing art: In the brain of the beholder. Oxford: Oxford University Press

/*ref*/Siebler, M. (2007). Greek art. Köln: Taschen

/*ref*/Solso, R. L. (1996). Cognition and the visual arts. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

/*ref*/olso, R. L. (2003). The psychology of art and the evolution of the conscious brain. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

/*ref*/Spivey, M. (2006). The continuity of mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press

/*ref*/Spivey, N. (2006). How art made the world: A journey to the origins of human creativity. New York. Basic Books.

/*ref*/Stark, L. W. & Ellis, S. R. (1981). Scanpath revisited: Cognitive models direct active looking. In D. F. Fisher, R. A. Monty & J.W. Senders (Eds.) Eye movements: Cognition and visual perception (pp. 193-226). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

/*ref*/Starr, G. (2013). Feeling beauty – The neuroscience of aesthetic experience. Cambridge: MIT Press.

/*ref*/Stefanakis, M.I. (2012). Introduction in classical archaeology. Basic principles and overview of the ancient greek art (11th-4th c.). Athens: Iamvlichos.

/*ref*/Stewart, A. (1977). To entertain an emperor: Sperlonga, Laokoon and Tiberius at the dinner-table, The Journal of Roman Studies, 67, 76–90. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/299920

/*ref*/Tong, F., Nakayama, K., Moscovitch, M., Weinrib, O., & Kanwisher, N. (2000). Response properties of the human fusiform face area. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 257–279.

/*ref*/Verstegen, I. (2005). Arnheim, gestalt and art: a psychological theory. Wien: Springer

/*ref*/Valavanis, P. (2004). Games and sanctuaries in ancient Greece - Olympia, Delphi, Isthmia, Nemea, Athens (D. Hardy, Trans.). Los Angeles: Getty Publications.

/*ref*/Vessel, E. A., Starr, G. G. & Rubin, N. (2012). The brain on art: intense aesthetic experience activates the default mode network. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 1-17. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00066

/*ref*/Vermeule, C. (1975). The weary Herakles of Lysippos, American Journal of Archaeology, 79(4), 323-332. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/503065

/*ref*/Yarbus, A. L. (1967). Eye movements and vision (B. Haigh, Trans.). New York: Plenum Press.

/*ref*/Yin, R.K. (1989). Case study research - Design and methods, rev. ed. Newsbury Park: Sage Publications.

/*ref*/Zaidel, D. W. (2013). Art and brain: The relationship of biology and evolution to art. In S. Finger, D. W. Zaidel, F. Boller & J. Bogousslavsky (Eds.). The fine arts, neurology and neuroscience – New discoveries and changing landscapes (pp. 217– 234). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Direitos

LICENCE OF USE: The full text articles included on the Scientific Journals of the Complutense website are open access and the property of their authors and/or publishers. Therefore, any reproduction, distribution, public communication and/or total or partial transformation requires their express and written consent. Links to the full text of the articles on the Scientific Journals of the Complutense website should be to the official URL of the Complutense University of Madrid.

La revista Arte, Individuo y Sociedad, para fomentar el intercambio global del conocimiento, facilita el acceso sin restricciones a sus contenidos desde el momento de su publicación en la presente edición electrónica, y por eso es una revista de acceso abierto. Los originales publicados en esta revista son propiedad de la Universidad Complutense de Madrid y es obligatorio citar su procedencia en cualquier reproducción total o parcial. Todos los contenidos se distribuyen bajo una licencia de uso y distribución Creative Commons Reconocimiento 4.0 (CC BY 4.0). Esta circunstancia ha de hacerse constar expresamente de esta forma cuando sea necesario. Puede consultar la versión informativa y el texto legal de la licencia.La revista Arte, Individuo y Sociedad no cobra por tasas por envío de trabajos, ni tampoco cuotas por la publicación de sus artículos.

Fonte

Arte, Individuo y Sociedad; Vol 27, No 3 (2015); 517-535

Palavras-Chave #Ciencias Sociales > Psicología > Psicología del Arte #attention; ancient Greek art; selective attention; psychology of art #atención; atención selectiva; arte Griego antiguo; psicología del arte
Tipo

info:eu-repo/semantics/article

Artículo revisado por pares

info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion